1887
Volume 14, Issue 4
  • ISSN 1018-2101
  • E-ISSN: 2406-4238

Abstract

A review of the current literature shows that by the age of two and a half (and probably earlier), children have already acquired a rich working knowledge of human intentionality and goal-directed action (Stein & Albro 1997: 7; Mandler 1998). The paper focuses on the ways in which children use this knowledge to tell stories from pictures. The story is the description of the actions performed by animate actors. We distinguish the main actors (protagonists in the narrative line) and the background actors (participants in the narrative field) who can observe and interpret what is going on in the main action. So the narrative text contains not only the action presented by the story-teller (landscape of action) but also how this action is interpreted by the story characters (landscape of consciousness). They are all thinking minds who can think similarly or differently about the plot. And the narrator uses characters’ minds to produce different representations of the story (Bokus 1998, 2000). The narrator can confront one interpretation with another, and a) makes choices of the “true” representation of the main action (in doing this the child plays the role of the omniscient and omnipresent story-teller who is directly in touch with the ontology of the story), or b) presents a possible but not a certain story reality (the listener is not told how things are but rather how they seem to be). Therefore we can speak about the interplay of the narrator’s mind and the minds of story characters in a kind of internal narrator’s dialogue. The storyteller creates different minds and alternative ways of interpreting the main action. Also shown are examples of such inter-mind phenomena in the stories told by preschool children.

Loading

Article metrics loading...

/content/journals/10.1075/prag.14.4.01bok
2004-01-01
2024-12-04
Loading full text...

Full text loading...

References

  1. Abbeduto, L. , and S. Rosenberg
    (1985) Children’s knowledge of the presuppositions of know and other verbs. Journal of Child Language12: 621-641. doi: 10.1017/S0305000900006693
    https://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000900006693 [Google Scholar]
  2. Astington, J.W
    (1990) Narrative and the child’s theory of mind. In B.K. Britton , & A.D. Pellegrini (eds.), Narrative thought and narrative language. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum, pp.151-171.
    [Google Scholar]
  3. (1993) The child’s discovery of the mind. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  4. Bakhtin, M
    (1984) Problems of Dostoevski’s poetics. Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  5. Bamberg, M
    (ed.) (1997) Narrative development: Six approaches. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
    [Google Scholar]
  6. Bamberg, M. , and R. Damrad-Frye
    (1991) On the ability to provide evaluative comments: Further explorations of children’s narrative competence. Journal of Child Language18: 689-710. doi: 10.1017/S0305000900011314
    https://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000900011314 [Google Scholar]
  7. Bartsch, K. , and H.M. Wellman
    (1995) Children talk about the mind. New York: Oxford University Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  8. Bokus, B
    (1991) Children’s pragmatic knowledge of narrative tasks. In J. Verschueren (ed.), Pragmatics at issue. Vol. 1 (Selected papers of the International Pragmatics Conference, Antwerp – 1987). Amsterdam: John Benjamins Publishing Company, pp.13-28. doi: 10.1075/pbns.6.1.03bok
    https://doi.org/10.1075/pbns.6.1.03bok [Google Scholar]
  9. (1992) Peer co-narration: Changes in structure of preschoolers’ participation. Journal of Narrative and Life History2: 253-275.
    [Google Scholar]
  10. (1996a) Action and consciousness: Duality of children’s narrative construction. Paper presented at theVIIth International Congress for the Study of Child Language (Istanbul, July 14–19, 1996).
    [Google Scholar]
  11. (1996b) Narrative space structuring at the preschool age (Findings on monologic and dialogic discourse. In C.E. Johnson & J.H.V. Gilbert (eds.), Children’s Language, vol. 9. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum, pp.197-207.
    [Google Scholar]
  12. (1998) Action and its representation in the minds of story characters. Findings from children’s discourse. Psychology of Language and Communication2.2: 63-77.
    [Google Scholar]
  13. (2000) Światy fabuły w narracji dziecięcej (Story worlds in child narration). Warsaw: Energeia.
    [Google Scholar]
  14. Britton, B.K. , and A.D. Pellegrini
    (eds.) (1990) Narrative thought and narrative language. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
    [Google Scholar]
  15. Bruner, J
    (1986) Actual minds, possible worlds. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  16. (1990) Acts of meaning. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  17. Donaldson, M
    (1978) Children’s minds. New York: Norton.
    [Google Scholar]
  18. Eco, U
    (1994) Lector in fabula. Warszawa: PIW.
    [Google Scholar]
  19. Ely, R ., A. McCabe , A. Wolf , and G. Melzi
    (2000) The story behind the story: Gathering narrative data from children. In L. Menn , & N.B. Ratner (eds.), Methods for studying language production. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum, pp.249-269.
    [Google Scholar]
  20. Flavell, J.H. , and P.H. Miller
    (1998) Social cognition. In D. Kuhn , & R. Siegler (eds.), Cognition, perception, and language. Vol. II of W. Damon (Gen.ed.), Handbook of child psychology. New York: Wiley, pp.851-898.
    [Google Scholar]
  21. Fox, R
    (1991) Developing awareness of mind reflected in children’s narrative writing. British Journal of Developmental Psychology9: 281-298. doi: 10.1111/j.2044‑835X.1991.tb00877.x
    https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-835X.1991.tb00877.x [Google Scholar]
  22. Gopnik, A. , and A.N. Meltzoff
    (1997) Words, thoughts, and theories. Cambridge MA: MIT Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  23. Halliday, M.A.K. , and R. Hasan
    (1976) Cohesion in English. London: Longman.
    [Google Scholar]
  24. Hermans, H.J.M
    (1997) Self-narrative in the life course: A contextual approach. In M. Bamberg (ed.), Narrative development: Six approaches. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum, pp.223-264.
    [Google Scholar]
  25. Hudson, J.A. , and L.R. Shapiro
    (1991) From kowing to telling: The development of children’s scripts, stories, and personal narratives. In A. McCabe , & C. Peterson (eds.), Developing narrative structure. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum, pp.89-136.
    [Google Scholar]
  26. Kielar-Turska, M
    (1999) The inner landscape of characters in stories told by children. Psychology of Language and Communication3.2: 49-56.
    [Google Scholar]
  27. Labov, W. , and J. Waletzky
    (1967) Narrative analysis: Oral versions of personal experience. In J. Helm (ed.), Essays on the verbal and visual arts. Seattle: University of Washington Press, pp.12-44.
    [Google Scholar]
  28. Lewis, C. , N.H. Freeman, C. Hagestadt , and H. Douglas
    (1994) Narrative access and production in preschoolers’ false belief reasoning. Cognitive Development9: 397-424. doi: 10.1016/0885‑2014(94)90013‑2
    https://doi.org/10.1016/0885-2014(94)90013-2 [Google Scholar]
  29. Lucariello, J
    (1990) Canonicality and consciousness in child narrative. In B.K. Britton , & A.D. Pellegrini (eds.), Narrative thought and narrative language. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum, pp.149.
    [Google Scholar]
  30. Lyons, J
    (1977) Semantics, vol. 2. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  31. Macnamara, J. , E. Baker , and C. Olson
    (1976) Four-year-olds’ understanding of pretend, forget and know: Evidence for propositional operations. Child Development47: 62-70. doi: 10.2307/1128458
    https://doi.org/10.2307/1128458 [Google Scholar]
  32. Mandler, J.M
    (1998) Representation. In D. Kuhn , & R.S. Siegler (eds.), Cognition, perception, and language: Vol. 2, W. Damon (ed.), Handbook of child psychology . New York: Wiley, pp.255-281.
    [Google Scholar]
  33. Musatti, T
    (1986) Early peer relations: The perspectives of Piaget and Vygotsky. In E.C. Mueller , & C.R. Cooper (eds.), Process and outcome in peer relationships. Orlando: Academic Press, pp.25-53.
    [Google Scholar]
  34. Nicolopoulou, A. , and E.S. Richner
    (1999a) From actors to characters to persons: The development of character representation in young children’s narratives. Poster presented at the1999 Biennial Meeting of the Society for Research in Child Development. Albuquerque (April 15-18, 1999), (work in progress), NM.
    [Google Scholar]
  35. (1999b) The development of main character in young children’s narratives. Paper presented at theVIIIth Congress for the Study of Child Language, San Sebastian (July 12–16, 1999), NM.
    [Google Scholar]
  36. Pillow, B.H
    (1989) Early understanding of perception as a source of knowledge. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology47: 116-129. doi: 10.1016/0022‑0965(89)90066‑0
    https://doi.org/10.1016/0022-0965(89)90066-0 [Google Scholar]
  37. Pratt, C. , and P. Bryant
    (1990) Young children understand that looking leads to knowing. Child Development61: 973-982. doi: 10.2307/1130869
    https://doi.org/10.2307/1130869 [Google Scholar]
  38. Searle, J.R
    (1983) Intentionality: An essay in the philosophy of mind. New York: Cambridge University Press. doi: 10.1017/CBO9781139173452
    https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9781139173452 [Google Scholar]
  39. (2002) Consciousness and language. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. doi: 10.1017/CBO9780511606366
    https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511606366 [Google Scholar]
  40. Shields, M
    (1978) The child as psychologist: Construing the social world. In A. Lock (ed.), Action, gesture, and symbol: The emergence of language. London: Academic Press, pp.529-561.
    [Google Scholar]
  41. Shugar, G.W
    (1976) Text-constructing with an adult. A form of child activity during early language acquisition. In D. Drachman (ed.), Akten des 1. Salzburger Kolloquiums über Kindersprache. Tübingen: Verlag Gunter Narr, pp.343-356.
    [Google Scholar]
  42. (1998) Entry into narration: Uses of operation of reference. Psychology of Language and Communication2.2: 7-16.
    [Google Scholar]
  43. Stein, N.L. , and E.R. Albro
    (1997) Building complexity and coherence: Children’s use of goal-structured knowledge in telling stories. In M. Bamberg (ed.), Narrative development: Six approaches. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum, pp.5-44.
    [Google Scholar]
  44. Taylor, M
    (1996) A theory of mind perspective on social development. In R. Gelman , & T.K.-F. Au (eds.), Perceptual and cognitive development. San Diego, CA: Academic Press, pp.283-329. doi: 10.1016/B978‑012279660‑9/50027‑0
    https://doi.org/10.1016/B978-012279660-9/50027-0 [Google Scholar]
  45. Tomasello, M
    (1999) The cultural origins of human cognition. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  46. Tomaszewski, T
    (1975) Człowiek i otoczenie (Man and environment). In T. Tomaszewski (ed.), Psychologia. Warsaw: PWN, pp.13-36.
    [Google Scholar]
  47. Wertsch, J.V
    (1991) Voices of the mind. A sociocultural approach to mediated action. Cambridge, Mass.: Harvard University Press.
    [Google Scholar]
/content/journals/10.1075/prag.14.4.01bok
Loading
This is a required field
Please enter a valid email address
Approval was successful
Invalid data
An Error Occurred
Approval was partially successful, following selected items could not be processed due to error