1887
Volume 33, Issue 3
  • ISSN 1018-2101
  • E-ISSN: 2406-4238

Abstract

Abstract

The idea that interpreting a lexeme typically involves a context-dependent process of meaning construction has in recent years become common ground in linguistic theory. This view is very explicit in relevance theory (Sperber and Wilson 1995), which posits that speakers systematically infer ad hoc concepts (Carston 2002). Such an approach raises theoretical issues, though. First, it directly poses a challenge for the exact nature of (and difference between) concepts and ad hoc concepts (Carston 2002, 249). In addition, as Wilson (20112016) and Carston (20132016) point out, this view also uncovers the following paradox: if speakers are assumed to follow a path of least effort (relevance heuristics), why should they so systematically infer ad hoc concepts rather than test the encoded concept first? The aim of this paper is to reflect on this theoretical puzzle. It will first be argued that the hypotheses formulated both by Wilson and by Carston seem rather post hoc and fail to fully resolve the apparent paradox. Attention will then be given to the assumed nature of (ad hoc) concepts to show that the problem can be resolved when an alternative (non-atomic) view of concepts in terms of meaning potential is adopted.

Loading

Article metrics loading...

/content/journals/10.1075/prag.21057.lec
2022-11-07
2024-12-10
Loading full text...

Full text loading...

/deliver/fulltext/prag.21057.lec.html?itemId=/content/journals/10.1075/prag.21057.lec&mimeType=html&fmt=ahah

References

  1. Allwood, Jens
    2003 “Meaning Potential and Context: Some Consequences for the Analysis of Variation in Meaning.” InCognitive Approaches to Lexical Semantics, ed. byHubert Cuyckens, René Dirven, and John R. Taylor, 29–65. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter. 10.1515/9783110219074.29
    https://doi.org/10.1515/9783110219074.29 [Google Scholar]
  2. Assimakopoulos, Stavros
    2008 “Intention, Common Ground and the Availability of Semantic Content: A Relevance-Theoretic Perspective.” InIntention, Common Ground and the Egocentric Speaker-Hearer, ed. byIstvan Kecskes, and Jacob Mey, 105–126. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter. 10.1515/9783110211474.1.105
    https://doi.org/10.1515/9783110211474.1.105 [Google Scholar]
  3. Barsalou, Lawrence W.
    1983 “Ad Hoc Categories.” Memory and Cognition111: 211–227. 10.3758/BF03196968
    https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03196968 [Google Scholar]
  4. 1987 “The Instability of Graded Structure: Implications for the Nature of Concepts.” InConcepts and Conceptual Development, ed. byUlric Neisser, 101–140. New York: Cambridge University Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  5. 1993 “Flexibility, Structure, and Linguistic Vagary in Concepts: Manifestations of a Compositional System of Perceptual Symbols.” InTheories of Memory, ed. byAlan F. Collins, Susan E. Gathercole, Martin A. Conway, and Peter E. Morris, 29–101. Hove: Lawrence Erlbaum.
    [Google Scholar]
  6. 2000 “Concepts: Structure.” InEncyclopedia of Psychology, vol.21, ed. byAlan E. Kazdin, 245–248. New York: Oxford University Press. 10.1037/10517‑091
    https://doi.org/10.1037/10517-091 [Google Scholar]
  7. 2012 “The Human Conceptual System.” InThe Cambridge Handbook of Psycholinguistics, ed. byMichael J. Spivey, Ken McRae, and Marc F. Joanisse, 239–258. New York: Cambridge University Press. 10.1017/CBO9781139029377.013
    https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9781139029377.013 [Google Scholar]
  8. 2016 “Situated Conceptualization: Theory and Applications.” InFoundations of Embodied Cognition, vol.11: Perceptual and Emotional Embodiment, ed. byYann Coello, and Martin H. Fischer, 11–37. Routledge: Psychology Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  9. Bezuidenhout, Anne
    2002 “Truth-Conditional Pragmatics.” Philosophical Perspectives161: 105–134.
    [Google Scholar]
  10. Borg, Emma
    2016 “Exploding Explicatures.” Mind & Language311: 335–355. 10.1111/mila.12109
    https://doi.org/10.1111/mila.12109 [Google Scholar]
  11. Carston, Robyn
    2002Thoughts and Utterances: The Pragmatics of Explicit Communication. Oxford: Blackwell. 10.1002/9780470754603
    https://doi.org/10.1002/9780470754603 [Google Scholar]
  12. 2010 “Explicit Communication and ‘Free’ Pragmatic Enrichment.” InExplicit Communication: Robyn Carston’s Pragmatics, ed. byBelén Soria, and Esther Romero, 271–285. Basingtoke: Palgrave Macmillan. 10.1057/9780230292352_14
    https://doi.org/10.1057/9780230292352_14 [Google Scholar]
  13. 2012 “Word Meaning and Concept Expressed.” The Linguistic Review291: 607–623. 10.1515/tlr‑2012‑0022
    https://doi.org/10.1515/tlr-2012-0022 [Google Scholar]
  14. 2013 “Word Meaning, What Is Said and Explicature.” InWhat Is Said and What Is Not, ed. byCarolo Penco, and Filippo Domaneschi, 175–203. Stanford: CSLI Publications.
    [Google Scholar]
  15. 2016 “The Heterogeneity of Procedural Meaning.” Lingua1751: 154–166. 10.1016/j.lingua.2015.12.010
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lingua.2015.12.010 [Google Scholar]
  16. 2019 “Ad Hoc Concepts, Polysemy and the Lexicon.” InRelevance, Pragmatics and Interpretation, ed. byKate Scott, Billy Clark, and Robyn Carston, 150–162. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. 10.1017/9781108290593.014
    https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108290593.014 [Google Scholar]
  17. 2021 “Polysemy: Pragmatics and Sense Conventions.” Mind & Language361: 108–133. 10.1111/mila.12329
    https://doi.org/10.1111/mila.12329 [Google Scholar]
  18. Croft, William, and D. Alan Cruse
    2004Cognitive Linguistics. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. 10.1017/CBO9780511803864
    https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511803864 [Google Scholar]
  19. Dobler, Tamara
    2020 “The Occasion-Sensitivity of Thought.” Topoi391: 487–497. 10.1007/s11245‑017‑9532‑2
    https://doi.org/10.1007/s11245-017-9532-2 [Google Scholar]
  20. Escandell-Vidal, Victoria
    2017 “Notes for a Restrictive Theory of Procedural Meaning.” InDoing Pragmatics Interculturally: Cognitive, Philosophical, and Sociopragmatic Perspectives, ed. byRachel Giora, and Michael Haugh, 79–95. Berlin: Walter de Gruyter. 10.1515/9783110546095‑005
    https://doi.org/10.1515/9783110546095-005 [Google Scholar]
  21. Evans, Vyvyan, and Melanie Green
    2006Cognitive Linguistics: An Introduction. Edinburgh: Edinburgh University Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  22. Falkum, Ingrid L.
    2011 “The Semantics and Pragmatics of Polysemy: A Relevance-Theoretic Account.” PhD diss.University College London.
    [Google Scholar]
  23. Fauconnier, Gilles, and Mark Turner
    2003 “Polysemy and Conceptual Blending.” InPolysemy: Flexible Patterns of Meaning in Mind and Language, ed. byBrigitte Nerlich, Zazie Todd, Vimala Herman, and David Clarke, 79–94. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter. 10.1515/9783110895698.79
    https://doi.org/10.1515/9783110895698.79 [Google Scholar]
  24. Fillmore, Charles J.
    1982 “Frame Semantics.” InLinguistics in the Morning Calm, ed. byThe Linguistic Society of Korea, 111–138. Seoul: Hanshin.
    [Google Scholar]
  25. Fodor, Jerry A.
    1975The Language of Thought. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  26. 2001 “Language, Thought and Compositionality.” Mind & Language161: 1–15. 10.1111/1468‑0017.00153
    https://doi.org/10.1111/1468-0017.00153 [Google Scholar]
  27. Gibbs, Raymond W., Jr.
    1994The Poetics of Mind: Figurative Thought, Language, and Understanding. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  28. Groefsema, Marjolein
    2007 “Concepts and Word Meaning in Relevance Theory.” InPragmatics, ed. byNoël Burton-Roberts, 136–157. Basingtoke: Palgrave Macmillan. 10.1057/978‑1‑349‑73908‑0_8
    https://doi.org/10.1057/978-1-349-73908-0_8 [Google Scholar]
  29. Haiman, John
    1980 “Dictionaries and Encyclopedias.” Lingua501: 329–357. 10.1016/0024‑3841(80)90089‑3
    https://doi.org/10.1016/0024-3841(80)90089-3 [Google Scholar]
  30. Hall, Alison
    2017 “Lexical Pragmatics, Explicature and Ad Hoc Concepts.” InSemantics and Pragmatics: Drawing a Line, ed. byIlse Depraetere, and Raphael Salkie, 85–100. Berlin: Springer International Publishing. 10.1007/978‑3‑319‑32247‑6_6
    https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-32247-6_6 [Google Scholar]
  31. Halliday, Michael A. K.
    1973Explorations in the Functions of Language. London: Edward Arnold.
    [Google Scholar]
  32. Horsey, Richard
    2006 “The Content and Acquisition of Lexical Concepts.” PhD diss.University College London.
    [Google Scholar]
  33. Lakoff, George
    1987Women, Fire, and Dangerous Things: What Categories Reveal about the Mind. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. 10.7208/chicago/9780226471013.001.0001
    https://doi.org/10.7208/chicago/9780226471013.001.0001 [Google Scholar]
  34. Langacker, Ronald W.
    1987Foundations of Cognitive Grammar, vol.11: Theoretical Prerequisites. Stanford: Stanford University Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  35. Leclercq, Benoît
    2019a “On the Semantics-Pragmatics Interface: A Theoretical Bridge between Construction Grammar and Relevance Theory.” PhD diss.University of Lille.
    [Google Scholar]
  36. 2019b “Coercion: A Case of Saturation.” Constructions and Frames11 (2): 270–289. 10.1075/cf.00031.lec
    https://doi.org/10.1075/cf.00031.lec [Google Scholar]
  37. 2020 “Semantics and Pragmatics in Construction Grammar.” Belgian Journal of Linguistics341: 225–234. 10.1075/bjl.00048.lec
    https://doi.org/10.1075/bjl.00048.lec [Google Scholar]
  38. Lemmens, Maarten
    2017 “A Cognitive, Usage-Based View on Lexical Pragmatics: Response to Hall.” InSemantics and Pragmatics: Drawing a Line, ed. byIlse Depraetere, and Raphael Salkie, 101–114. Berlin: Springer International Publishing. 10.1007/978‑3‑319‑32247‑6_7
    https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-32247-6_7 [Google Scholar]
  39. Murphy, Gregory L.
    1991 “Meaning and Concepts.” InThe Psychology of Word Meaning, ed. byPaula J. Schwanenflugel, 11–35. Hillsdale: Erlbaum.
    [Google Scholar]
  40. Norén, Kerstin, and Per Linell
    2007 “Meaning Potentials and the Interaction Between Lexis and Contexts: An Empirical Substantiation.” Pragmatics171: 387–416.
    [Google Scholar]
  41. Pustejovsky, James
    1995The Generative Lexicon. Cambridge: MIT Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  42. Reboul, Anne
    2000 “Words, Concepts, Mental Representations, and Other Biological Categories.” InThe Lexicon-Encyclopedia Interface, ed. byBert Peeters, 55–95. Amsterdam: Elsevier. 10.1163/9780585474465_003
    https://doi.org/10.1163/9780585474465_003 [Google Scholar]
  43. Recanati, François
    2004Literal Meaning. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  44. Reddy, Michael
    1979 “The Conduit Metaphor: A Case of Conflict in Our Language about Language.” InMetaphor and Thought, ed. byAndrew Ortony, 284–324. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  45. Sperber, Dan
    2005 “Modularity and Relevance: How Can a Massively Modular Mind Be Flexible and Context-Sensitive?” InThe Innate Mind: Structure and Content, ed. byPeter Carruthers, and Stephen Laurence, 53–68. Oxford: Oxford University Press. 10.1093/acprof:oso/9780195179675.003.0004
    https://doi.org/10.1093/acprof:oso/9780195179675.003.0004 [Google Scholar]
  46. Sperber, Dan, and Deirdre Wilson
    1987 “Precis of Relevance: Communication and Cognition.” Behavioural and Brain Sciences101: 697–754. 10.1017/S0140525X00055345
    https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X00055345 [Google Scholar]
  47. 1995Relevance: Communication and Cognition. 2nd ed.Oxford: Blackwell.
    [Google Scholar]
  48. 1998 “The Mapping between the Mental and the Public Lexicon.” InLanguage and Thought: Interdisciplinary Themes, ed. byPeter Carruthers, and Jill Boucher, 184–200. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. 10.1017/CBO9780511597909.012
    https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511597909.012 [Google Scholar]
  49. 2008 “A Deflationary Account of Metaphor.” InThe Cambridge Handbook of Metaphor and Thought, ed. byRaymond Gibbs, 84–105. New York: Cambridge University Press. 10.1017/CBO9780511816802.007
    https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511816802.007 [Google Scholar]
  50. Verschueren, Jef
    2018 “Adaptability and Meaning Potential.” InThe Dynamics of Language, ed. byRajend Mesthrie, and David Bradley, 99–109. Cape Town: UCT Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  51. Wilson, Deirdre
    1995 “Is There a Maxim of Truthfulness?” UCL Working Papers in Linguistics71: 197–212.
    [Google Scholar]
  52. 2003 “Relevance Theory and Lexical Pragmatics.” Italian Journal of Linguistics/Rivista di Linguistica151: 273–291.
    [Google Scholar]
  53. 2009 “Parallels and Differences in the Treatment of Metaphor in Relevance Theory and Cognitive Linguistics.” Studies in Pragmatics111: 42–60.
    [Google Scholar]
  54. 2011 “The Conceptual-Procedural Distinction: Past, Present and Future.” InProcedural Meaning: Problems and Perspectives, ed. byVictoria Escandell-Vidal, Manuel Leonetti, and Aoife Ahern, 3–31. Bingley: Emerald Group Publishing. 10.1108/S1472‑7870(2011)0000025005
    https://doi.org/10.1108/S1472-7870(2011)0000025005 [Google Scholar]
  55. 2016 “Reassessing the Conceptual-Procedural Distinction.” Lingua1751: 5–19. 10.1016/j.lingua.2015.12.005
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lingua.2015.12.005 [Google Scholar]
  56. Wilson, Deirdre, and Robyn Carston
    2006 “Metaphor, Relevance and the ‘Emergent Property’ Issue.” Mind and Language211: 404–433. 10.1111/j.1468‑0017.2006.00284.x
    https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1468-0017.2006.00284.x [Google Scholar]
  57. 2007 “A Unitary Approach to Lexical Pragmatics: Relevance, Inference and Ad Hoc Concepts.” InPragmatics, ed. byNoël Burton-Roberts, 230–259. Basingtoke: Palgrave Macmillan. 10.1057/978‑1‑349‑73908‑0_12
    https://doi.org/10.1057/978-1-349-73908-0_12 [Google Scholar]
  58. Wilson, Deirdre, and Dan Sperber
    2004 “Relevance Theory.” InThe Handbook of Pragmatics, ed. byLaurence R. Horn, and Gregory Ward, 607–632. Oxford: Blackwell.
    [Google Scholar]
/content/journals/10.1075/prag.21057.lec
Loading
  • Article Type: Research Article
Keyword(s): ad hoc concepts; meaning potential; paradox; relevance heuristics; relevance theory
This is a required field
Please enter a valid email address
Approval was successful
Invalid data
An Error Occurred
Approval was partially successful, following selected items could not be processed due to error