1887
Volume 16, Issue 1
  • ISSN 1877-9751
  • E-ISSN: 1877-976X
USD
Buy:$35.00 + Taxes

Abstract

This paper investigates how a mental-model theory of communication can explain differences in humorous texts and how aesthetic criteria to evaluate humour are dependent on the way mental models are exploited. Humour is defined as the deliberate manipulation by speakers of their private mental models of situations in order to create public mental models which contain one or more incongruities. Recipients can re-construct this manipulation process and thereby evaluate its nature and its quality. Humorous texts can be distinguished in terms of ownership of the manipulated mental model, the relationship between the speakers’ private and their public (humorous) mental model, as well as the speed required in the humorous mental model construction. Possible aesthetic criteria are the quality of the mental model manipulation, the pressure under which the humorously manipulated mental models have been constructed and the quality of the presentation of humorous mental models.

Loading

Article metrics loading...

/content/journals/10.1075/rcl.00005.jon
2018-05-31
2025-02-10
Loading full text...

Full text loading...

References

  1. Apter, M. J.
    (1982) The experience of motivation: The theory of psychological reversals. London: Academic Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  2. Apter, M. J. , & Desselles, M.
    (2012) Disclosure humor and distortion humor: A reversal theory analysis. Humor – International Journal of Humor Research, 25(4), 417–435.
    [Google Scholar]
  3. Attardo, S.
    (2000) Irony as relevant inappropriateness. Journal of Pragmatics, 32(6), 793–826.10.1016/S0378‑2166(99)00070‑3
    https://doi.org/10.1016/S0378-2166(99)00070-3 [Google Scholar]
  4. (2008) A primer for the linguistics of humor. In V. Raskin (Ed.), The primer of humor research (pp.101–156). Berlin/New York: Mouton de Gruyter.10.1515/9783110198492.101
    https://doi.org/10.1515/9783110198492.101 [Google Scholar]
  5. Baron-Cohen, S.
    (2001) Theory of mind in normal development and autism. Prisme, 34, 174–183.
    [Google Scholar]
  6. BBC
    BBC (2011) The office: 10th anniversary edition [DVD]. London: BBC Worldwide.
    [Google Scholar]
  7. Bent, S. A.
    (1887) Short sayings of great men: With historical and explanatory notes (classic reprint) (sixth edition, revised and enlarged). Boston: Ticknor and Co.
    [Google Scholar]
  8. Boxer, D. , & Cortés-Conde, F.
    (1997) From bonding to biting: Conversational joking and identity display. Journal of Pragmatics, 27(3), 275–294.10.1016/S0378‑2166(96)00031‑8
    https://doi.org/10.1016/S0378-2166(96)00031-8 [Google Scholar]
  9. Chafe, W. L.
    (2007) The importance of not being earnest: The feeling behind laughter and humor. Amsterdam/Philadelphia: John Benjamins.10.1075/ceb.3
    https://doi.org/10.1075/ceb.3 [Google Scholar]
  10. Chiaro, D.
    (1992) The language of jokes: Analyzing verbal play. London: Routledge.10.4324/9780203327562
    https://doi.org/10.4324/9780203327562 [Google Scholar]
  11. de Jongste, H.
    (2013) Negotiating humorous intent. In M. Dynel (Ed.), Developments in linguistic humour theory (pp.179–210). Amsterdam/Philadelphia: John Benjamins.10.1075/thr.1.09jon
    https://doi.org/10.1075/thr.1.09jon [Google Scholar]
  12. (2016) Mental models and humorous intent. Journal of Pragmatics, 95, 107–119.10.1016/j.pragma.2016.01.006
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pragma.2016.01.006 [Google Scholar]
  13. Dennett, D. C.
    (1987) The intentional stance. Cambridge, Mass: MIT Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  14. Dunbar, R. I. M.
    (1998) Theory of mind and the evolution of language. In J. R. Hurford , M. Studdert-Kennedy , & C. Knight (Eds.), Approaches to the evolution of language: Social and cognitive bases (pp.92–110). Cambridge, UK/New York: Cambridge University Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  15. Dynel, M.
    (2009) Beyond a joke: Types of conversational humour. Language and Linguistics Compass, 3(5), 1284–1299.10.1111/j.1749‑818X.2009.00152.x
    https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1749-818X.2009.00152.x [Google Scholar]
  16. (2011) Joker in the pack: Towards determining the status of humorous framing in conversations. In M. Dynel (Ed.), The pragmatics of humour across discourse domains (pp.217–241). Amsterdam/Philadelphia: John Benjamins.10.1075/pbns.210.15dyn
    https://doi.org/10.1075/pbns.210.15dyn [Google Scholar]
  17. (2013) Impoliteness as disaffiliative humour in film talk. In M. Dynel (Ed.), Developments in linguistic humour theory (pp.105–144). Amsterdam/Philadelphia: John Benjamins.10.1075/thr.1.07dyn
    https://doi.org/10.1075/thr.1.07dyn [Google Scholar]
  18. (2014) Isn’t it ironic?: Defining the scope of humorous irony. Humor, 27(4), 619–639.10.1515/humor‑2014‑0096
    https://doi.org/10.1515/humor-2014-0096 [Google Scholar]
  19. (2017) Academics vs. American scriptwriters vs. Academics: A battle over the etic and emic “sarcasm” and “irony” labels. Language & Communication, 55, 69–87.10.1016/j.langcom.2016.07.008
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.langcom.2016.07.008 [Google Scholar]
  20. Dynel, M. , Brock, A. , & de Jongste, H.
    (2016) A burgeoning field of research: Humorous intent in interaction. Journal of Pragmatics, 95, 51–57.10.1016/j.pragma.2016.02.004
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pragma.2016.02.004 [Google Scholar]
  21. Gallese, V. , & Goldman, A.
    (1998) Mirror neurons and the simulation theory of mind-reading. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 2(12), 493–501.10.1016/S1364‑6613(98)01262‑5
    https://doi.org/10.1016/S1364-6613(98)01262-5 [Google Scholar]
  22. Gervais, R. , & Merchant, S.
    (2003) The office: The scripts, series 1. London: BBC.
    [Google Scholar]
  23. Gordon, M.
    (2012) Exploring the relationship between humor and aesthetic experience. The Journal of Aesthetic Education, 46(1), 110–121.10.5406/jaesteduc.46.1.0110
    https://doi.org/10.5406/jaesteduc.46.1.0110 [Google Scholar]
  24. Haugh, M.
    (2008) Intention in pragmatics. Intercultural Pragmatics, 5(2), 99–110.10.1515/IP.2008.006
    https://doi.org/10.1515/IP.2008.006 [Google Scholar]
  25. (2012) On understandings of intention: A response to Wedgwood. Intercultural Pragmatics, 9(2), 161–194.10.1515/ip‑2012‑0011
    https://doi.org/10.1515/ip-2012-0011 [Google Scholar]
  26. Iacoboni, M.
    (2009) Mirroring people: The science of empathy and how we connect with others. New York: Farrar, Straus and Giroux.
    [Google Scholar]
  27. Kerr, J. H. , & Apter, M. J.
    (Eds.) (1991) Adult play: A reversal theory approach. Rockland, MA: Swets & Zeitlinger.
    [Google Scholar]
  28. Koestler, A.
    (1964) The act of creation. London: Pan Books.
    [Google Scholar]
  29. Kotthoff, H.
    (2007) Oral genres of humor: On the dialectic of genre knowledge and creative authoring. Pragmatics, 17, 263–296.10.1075/prag.17.2.04kot
    https://doi.org/10.1075/prag.17.2.04kot [Google Scholar]
  30. (2009) Joint construction of humorous fictions in conversation: An unnamed narrative activity in a playful keying. Journal of Literary Theory, 3(2), 195–218.10.1515/JLT.2009.012
    https://doi.org/10.1515/JLT.2009.012 [Google Scholar]
  31. Leech, G. N.
    (1983) Principles of pragmatics. London: Longman.
    [Google Scholar]
  32. Macdonald, N.
    (2015) The moth joke: Video fragment from a Conan O’Brien show published on YouTube. RetrievedJune 11, 2017, fromhttps://www.youtube.com/watch?v=eE6QzDrT_x8&list=PLyBqJL0ng4xR4hxVk1OQO42e0GLrer7Bw.
    [Google Scholar]
  33. Mahy, C. E. V. , Moses, L. J. , & Pfeifer, J. H.
    (2014) How and where: theory-of-mind in the brain. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 9, 68–81.10.1016/j.dcn.2014.01.002
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dcn.2014.01.002 [Google Scholar]
  34. Malle, B. F.
    (2002) The relation between language and theory of mind in development and evolution. In T. Givón & B. F. Malle (Eds.), The evolution of language out of pre-language (pp.265–284). Amsterdam/Philadelphia: John Benjamins.10.1075/tsl.53.14mal
    https://doi.org/10.1075/tsl.53.14mal [Google Scholar]
  35. Marsh, M.
    (2012) Foiled again: The playful ethics and aesthetics of jokes. Western Folklore, 71(3/4), 291–306.
    [Google Scholar]
  36. Matsumoto, D.
    (2007) Culture, context, and behavior. Journal of Personality, 75(6), 1285–1320.10.1111/j.1467‑6494.2007.00476.x
    https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-6494.2007.00476.x [Google Scholar]
  37. McLaughlin, M.
    (1981) The complete neurotic’s notebook. Secaucus, N.J.: Castle Books.
    [Google Scholar]
  38. Morreall, J.
    (1981) Humor and aesthetic education. Journal of Aesthetic Education, 15(1), 55–70.10.2307/3332209
    https://doi.org/10.2307/3332209 [Google Scholar]
  39. Norrick, N. R.
    (1993) Conversational joking: Humor in everyday talk. Bloomington: Indiana University Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  40. (2003) Issues in conversational joking. Journal of Pragmatics, 35(9), 1333–1359.10.1016/S0378‑2166(02)00180‑7
    https://doi.org/10.1016/S0378-2166(02)00180-7 [Google Scholar]
  41. Oring, E.
    (2016) Joking asides: The theory, analysis, and aesthetics of humor. Logan: Utah State University Press.10.7330/9781607324928
    https://doi.org/10.7330/9781607324928 [Google Scholar]
  42. Palmer, J.
    (1994) Taking humour seriously. London/New York: Routledge.10.4324/9780203380154
    https://doi.org/10.4324/9780203380154 [Google Scholar]
  43. Partington, A.
    (2006) The linguistics of laughter: A corpus-assisted study of laughter-talk. London: Routledge.
    [Google Scholar]
  44. Premack, D. , & Woodruff, G.
    (1978) Does the chimpanzee have a theory of mind?Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 1(4), 515–526.10.1017/S0140525X00076512
    https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X00076512 [Google Scholar]
  45. Priego-Valverde, B.
    (2006) How funny it is when everybody gets going!: A case of co-construction of humor in conversation. Círculo de Linguística Aplicada a la Comunicación, 27, 72–100.
    [Google Scholar]
  46. Sinkeviciute, V. , & Dynel, M.
    (2017) Approaching conversational humour culturally: A survey of the emerging area of investigation. Language & Communication, 55, 1–9.10.1016/j.langcom.2016.12.001
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.langcom.2016.12.001 [Google Scholar]
  47. Stallone, L. , & Haugh, M.
    (2017) Joint fantasising as relational practice in Brazilian Portuguese interactions. Language & Communication, 55, 10–23.10.1016/j.langcom.2016.08.012
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.langcom.2016.08.012 [Google Scholar]
  48. Tomasello, M.
    (2010) Origins of human communication. Cambridge, Mass, London: MIT Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  49. van Dijk, T. A.
    (2008) Discourse and context: A sociocognitive approach. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.10.1017/CBO9780511481499
    https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511481499 [Google Scholar]
  50. (2009) Society and discourse: How social contexts influence text and talk. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.10.1017/CBO9780511575273
    https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511575273 [Google Scholar]
  51. (2014) Discourse and knowledge: A sociocognitive approach. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.10.1017/CBO9781107775404
    https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9781107775404 [Google Scholar]
  52. Wilson, D. , & Sperber, D.
    (1994) Outline of relevance theory. Links & Letters, 1, 85–106.
    [Google Scholar]
/content/journals/10.1075/rcl.00005.jon
Loading
  • Article Type: Research Article
Keyword(s): aesthetic criterion; humour genre; mental model
This is a required field
Please enter a valid email address
Approval was successful
Invalid data
An Error Occurred
Approval was partially successful, following selected items could not be processed due to error