1887
Volume 20, Issue 2
  • ISSN 1877-9751
  • E-ISSN: 1877-976X
USD
Buy:$35.00 + Taxes

Abstract

Abstract

Despite the fact that semantic change studies have intensively argued that intensional readings develop from the literal reading as a whole, diachronic prototype semantics proposes that intensional readings arise from the extensional subsets of the literal reading. This study empirically explored this proposal by carrying out a corpus-based diachronic study. It is proved from the semantic change of Chinese that: (1) There exists a corresponding relationship between extensional usages and intensional readings of a lexical item. (2) Extension and intension both converge and diverge on semantic change. Their convergence lies in the fact that extensional usages give rise to intensional readings. Extensional usages, though nuanced, motivate the emergence and development of intensional readings. Their divergence is reflected in the independent development of extensional usages and intensional readings. The subsistence or dying out of extensional usages does not constrain the appearance or disappearance of intensional readings. (3) Semantic change involves three stages, namely the extensional stage, the intensional stage, and the grammaticalization stage. These three stages constitute an interweaving continuum in the process of semantic change.

Loading

Article metrics loading...

/content/journals/10.1075/rcl.00118.du
2022-12-08
2025-02-06
Loading full text...

Full text loading...

References

  1. Ancient Chinese Frequent-used Character Dictionary
    Ancient Chinese Frequent-used Character Dictionary (2019) Beijing: The Commercial Press.
  2. Bolinger, D.
    (1968) Entailment and the meaning of structures. Glossa, 21, 119–127.
    [Google Scholar]
  3. Brugman, C.
    (1981) The story of ‘over’. M.A. Thesis, Department of Linguistics, University of California Berkeley.
    [Google Scholar]
  4. (1988) The story of ‘Over’: Polysemy, semantics and the structure of the lexicon. New York: Garland.
    [Google Scholar]
  5. Campbell, L.
    (2004) Historical linguistics. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  6. Coleman, L., & Kay, P.
    (1981) Prototype semantics: The English word lie. Language, 57(1), 26–44. 10.1353/lan.1981.0002
    https://doi.org/10.1353/lan.1981.0002 [Google Scholar]
  7. Croft, W.
    (2000) Explaining language change: An evolutionary approach. Essex, U.K.: Longman, Pearson Education.
    [Google Scholar]
  8. (2006) The relevance of an evolutionary model to historical linguistics. InO. N. Thomsen (Ed.), Competing models of linguistic change: Evolution and beyond (pp.91–132). Amsterdam & Philadelphia: John Benjamins. 10.1075/cilt.279.08cro
    https://doi.org/10.1075/cilt.279.08cro [Google Scholar]
  9. Croft, W., & Cruse, A.
    (2004) Cognitive linguistics. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. 10.1017/CBO9780511803864
    https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511803864 [Google Scholar]
  10. Dik, S.
    (1977) Inductive generalizations in semantic change. InP. Hopper (Ed.), Studies in descriptive and historical linguistics: A Festschrift for Winfred P. Lehmann (pp. 283–300). Amsterdam & Philadelphia: John Benjamins. 10.1075/cilt.4.20dik
    https://doi.org/10.1075/cilt.4.20dik [Google Scholar]
  11. Divjak, D., & Arppe, A.
    (2013) Extracting prototypes from exemplars: What can corpus tell us about concept representation?Cognitive Linguistics, 24(2), 221–274. 10.1515/cog‑2013‑0008
    https://doi.org/10.1515/cog-2013-0008 [Google Scholar]
  12. Du, J., F. J. Li, & Xu, M.
    (2020) Pò (BREAK), qiē (CUT) and kāi (OPEN) in Chinese: A diachronic conceptual variational approach. Review of Cognitive Linguistics, 18(1), 213–243. 10.1075/rcl.00057.du
    https://doi.org/10.1075/rcl.00057.du [Google Scholar]
  13. Evans, V., & Green, M.
    (2006) Cognitive linguistics: An introduction. Edinburgh: Edinburgh University Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  14. Firth, J.
    (1957) Papers in linguistics. London: Oxford University Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  15. Geeraerts, D.
    (1983) Prototype theory and diachronic semantics: A case study. Indogermanische Forschungen, 881, 1–32.
    [Google Scholar]
  16. (1990) Meanings and prototypes. InS. L. Tsohatzidis (Eds.), Studies in linguistic categorization (pp. 195–210). London: Routledge.
    [Google Scholar]
  17. (1997) Diachronic prototype semantics: A contribution to historical lexicology. Oxford: Clarendon Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  18. (2006) The semantic structure of Dutch over. InD. Geeraerts (Eds.), Words and other wonders: Papers on lexical and semantic topics (pp.48–73). Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter. 10.1515/9783110219128.1.48
    https://doi.org/10.1515/9783110219128.1.48 [Google Scholar]
  19. (2007) Family resemblances, radial networks, and multidimensional models of meaning. InM. L. Friend, P. R. Vaz, S. H. Santano & J. Casanova (Eds.), Proceedings of the 30th AEDEAN Conference (pp. 1–11). Huelva: Servicio de Publicaciones de la Universidad de Huelva.
    [Google Scholar]
  20. (2010) Theories of lexical semantics. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  21. (2016) Prospects and problems of prototype theory. Diacronia, 31, 1–16. 10.17684/i4A53en
    https://doi.org/10.17684/i4A53en [Google Scholar]
  22. (Forthcoming). The structured nature of prepositional meaning. InW. A. Ross & S. Runge Eds. Cognitive linguistic approaches to Greek prepositions in the Bible. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  23. Geeraerts, D., & Cuyckens, H.
    (2007) Introducing cognitive linguistics. InD. Geeraerts & H. Cuyckens (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of Cognitive Linguistics (pp. 3–21). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  24. Grondelaers, S., Speelman, D., & Geeraerts, D.
    (2007) Lexical variation and change. InD. Geeraerts & H. Cuyckens (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of Cognitive Linguistics (pp.988–1011). New York: Oxford University Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  25. Grzega, J.
    (2003) On using (and misusing) prototypes for explanations of lexical changes. Word, 54(3): 335–357. 10.1080/00437956.2003.11432537
    https://doi.org/10.1080/00437956.2003.11432537 [Google Scholar]
  26. Harmon, Z., & Kapatsinski, V.
    (2017) Putting old tools to novel uses: The role of form accessibility in semantic extension. Cognitive Psychology, 981, 22–44. 10.1016/j.cogpsych.2017.08.002
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogpsych.2017.08.002 [Google Scholar]
  27. Harris, Z. S.
    (1954) Distributional structure. Word, 101, 146–162. 10.1080/00437956.1954.11659520
    https://doi.org/10.1080/00437956.1954.11659520 [Google Scholar]
  28. Heine, B., Claudi, U., & Hunnemeyer, F.
    (1991) From cognition to grammar: Evidence from African languages. InE. C. Traugott & B. Heine (Eds.), Approaches to grammaticalization (vol.11) (pp. 149–187). Amsterdam & Philadelphia: John Benjamins. 10.1075/tsl.19.1.09hei
    https://doi.org/10.1075/tsl.19.1.09hei [Google Scholar]
  29. Hock, H. H., & Joseph, B. D.
    (2009) Language history, language change, and language relationship: An introduction to historical and comparative linguistics. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter. 10.1515/9783110214307
    https://doi.org/10.1515/9783110214307 [Google Scholar]
  30. Hopper, P. J., & Traugott, E. C.
    (1993) Grammaticalization. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  31. Hu, C. R.
    (2005) The early forms of resultative construction and the relevant criterion. Studies of the Chinese Language, 3, 214–225. ( 胡敕瑞 2005 动结式的早期形式及判定标准. 中国语文(3): 214–225.)
    [Google Scholar]
  32. Jiang, S. Y.
    (1985) Lexical semantic development and change. Linguistic Researches, 21, 7–12. ( 蒋绍愚 1985 词义的发展和变化. 语文研究21, 7–12.)
    [Google Scholar]
  33. Kraska-Szlenk, I.
    (2014) Semantic extensions of body part terms: Common patterns and their interpretation. Language Sciences, 441, 15–39. 10.1016/j.langsci.2014.02.002
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.langsci.2014.02.002 [Google Scholar]
  34. Lakoff, G.
    (1987) Women, fire and dangerous things: What categories reveal about the mind. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. 10.7208/chicago/9780226471013.001.0001
    https://doi.org/10.7208/chicago/9780226471013.001.0001 [Google Scholar]
  35. Lander, Y., Maisak., T., & Rakhilina, E.
    (2008) Domains of aqua-motion: A case study in lexical typology. InE. van der Zee & M. Vulchanova (Eds.), Motion encoding in language and space (pp. 95–129). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  36. Langacker, R.
    (1987) Foundations of cognitive grammar, Volume I: Theoretical prerequisites. Stanford: Stanford University Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  37. Mervis, C. B., & Rosch, E.
    (1981) Categorization of natural objects. Annual Review of Psychology, 321, 89–115. 10.1146/annurev.ps.32.020181.000513
    https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.ps.32.020181.000513 [Google Scholar]
  38. Modern Chinese Dictionary
    Modern Chinese Dictionary (2016) Beijing: The Commercial Press.
  39. Norrick, N. R.
    (1981) Semiotic principles in semantic theory. Amsterdam studies in the theory and history of linguistic science IV. Amsterdam & Philadelphia: John Benjamins. 10.1075/cilt.20
    https://doi.org/10.1075/cilt.20 [Google Scholar]
  40. Peirsman, Y., & Geeraerts, D.
    (2006) Metonymy as a prototypical category. Cognitive Linguistics, 17(3), 269–316. 10.1515/COG.2006.007
    https://doi.org/10.1515/COG.2006.007 [Google Scholar]
  41. Rakhilina, E., & Reznikova, T.
    (2014) Doing lexical typology with frames and semantic maps. Basic Research Program Working Papers Series: Linguistics, WP BRP 18/LNG. 10.2139/ssrn.2564011
    https://doi.org/10.2139/ssrn.2564011 [Google Scholar]
  42. (2016) A frame-based methodology for lexical typology. InP. Juvonen. & M. Koptjevskaja-Tamm (Eds.), The lexical typology of semantic shifts (pp. 95–129). Berlin, Boston: Mouton de Gruyter. 10.1515/9783110377675‑004
    https://doi.org/10.1515/9783110377675-004 [Google Scholar]
  43. Robert, S.
    (2008) Words and their meanings: principles of variation and stabilization. InM. Vanhove (Eds.), From polysemy to semantic change (pp. 55–93). Amsterdam & Philadelphia: John Benjamins. 10.1075/slcs.106.05rob
    https://doi.org/10.1075/slcs.106.05rob [Google Scholar]
  44. Rosch, E.
    (1973) On the internal structure of perceptual and semantic categories. InT. E. Moore (Ed.), Cognitive development and the acquisition of language (pp.111–144). Academic. 10.1016/B978‑0‑12‑505850‑6.50010‑4
    https://doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-12-505850-6.50010-4 [Google Scholar]
  45. (1975) Cognitive representation of semantic categories. Journal of Experimental Psychology104(3), 192–233. 10.1037/0096‑3445.104.3.192
    https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-3445.104.3.192 [Google Scholar]
  46. (1978) Principles of categorization. InE. Rosch & B. B. Lloyd (Eds.), Cognition and categorization (pp. 27–48). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
    [Google Scholar]
  47. Rosch, E., & Mervis, C. B.
    (1975) Family resemblance: Studies in the internal structure of categories, Cognitive Psychology, 71, 573–605. 10.1016/0010‑0285(75)90024‑9
    https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0285(75)90024-9 [Google Scholar]
  48. Svorou, S.
    (1993) The grammar of space. Amsterdam & Philadelphia: Benjamins. 10.1075/tsl.25
    https://doi.org/10.1075/tsl.25 [Google Scholar]
  49. Sweetser, E.
    (1990) From etymology to pragmatics. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. 10.1017/CBO9780511620904
    https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511620904 [Google Scholar]
  50. Talmy, L.
    (2000) Toward a cognitive semantics, vol. II: Typology and process in concept structuring. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  51. Taylor, J.
    (2003) Linguistic categorization (3rd edition). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  52. Thomsen, O. N.
    (2006) Competing models of linguistic change: Evolution and beyond. Amsterdam & Philadelphia: John Benjamins. 10.1075/cilt.279
    https://doi.org/10.1075/cilt.279 [Google Scholar]
  53. Traugott, E. C.
    (2017) Semantic change. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  54. Traugott, E. C., & Dasher, R. B.
    (2002) Regularity in semantic change. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  55. Tyler, A., & Evans, V.
    (2001) Reconsidering prepositional polysemy networks: The case of over. Language77(4), 724–765. 10.1353/lan.2001.0250
    https://doi.org/10.1353/lan.2001.0250 [Google Scholar]
  56. Wang, L.
    (2013) Outline of the history of Chinese. Beijing: Zhong Hua Book Company. ( 王力 2013 汉语史稿. 北京:中华书局.)
    [Google Scholar]
  57. Weinreich, U., Labov, W., & Herzog, M. I.
    (1968) Empirical foundations for a theory of language Change. InW. P. Lehmann and Y. Malkiel (eds.), pp. 95–195. Directions for historical linguistics. Austin: University of Texas Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  58. Wu, F. X.
    (2017) The semantic changes of the Chinese locative term hou. Studies of the Chinese Language61, 494–506. ( 吴福祥 2017 汉语方所词语“后”的语义演变. 中国语文61, 494–506.)
    [Google Scholar]
  59. Xu, D.
    (2001) Investigating semantic influence on syntactic structure from the emergence of resultative constructions – a study on the divergence of the semantic and function of Chinese verbs. Linguistic Researches, 21, 5–12. ( 徐丹 2001 从动补结构的形成看语义对句法结构的影响—兼谈汉语动词语义及功能的分化. 语文研究21, 5–12.)
    [Google Scholar]
  60. (2005) Typological changes of some verbs in Chinese: The case of po (to break > broken). Studies of the Chinese Language, 4, 333–339. ( 徐丹 2005 谈“破”—汉语某些动词的类型转变. 中国语文 (4), 333–339.)
    [Google Scholar]
  61. Yu, N.
    (2008) Metaphor from body and culture. InR. W. Gibbs (Ed.), The Cambridge handbook of metaphor and thought (pp. 247–261). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. 10.1017/CBO9780511816802.016
    https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511816802.016 [Google Scholar]
/content/journals/10.1075/rcl.00118.du
Loading
/content/journals/10.1075/rcl.00118.du
Loading

Data & Media loading...

This is a required field
Please enter a valid email address
Approval was successful
Invalid data
An Error Occurred
Approval was partially successful, following selected items could not be processed due to error