1887
Applying Cognitive Linguistics
  • ISSN 1877-9751
  • E-ISSN: 1877-976X
USD
Buy:$35.00 + Taxes

Abstract

Ellis, O’Donnell, and Römer (2014) used free-association tasks to investigate knowledge of Verb-Argument Constructions (VACs). They demonstrated that English speakers have independent implicit knowledge of (i) verb frequency in the VAC, (ii) VAC-verb contingency, and (iii) verb prototypicality in terms of centrality within the VAC semantic network. They concluded that VAC processing involves rich associations, tuned by verb type and token frequencies and their contingencies of usage, which interface syntax, lexis, and semantics. However, the tasks they used, where respondents had a minute to think of the verbs that fitted in VAC frames like ‘he __ across the….’, ‘it __ of the….’, etc., were quite conscious and explicit. The current experiments therefore investigate the effects of these factors in on-line processing for recognition and naming. Experiment 1 tested the recognition of VAC exemplars from very brief, masked, visual presentations. Recognition threshold was affected by overall verb frequency in the language, by the frequency with which verbs appear in the VAC, and by VAC-verb contingency (ΔPcw). Experiment 2 had participants successively name VAC arguments as quickly as possible: first the VAC and then the preposition. Preposition naming latency was a function of verb frequency in the VAC. We consider the implications for the representation and processing of VACs.

Loading

Article metrics loading...

/content/journals/10.1075/rcl.14.1.05ell
2016-06-27
2025-02-16
Loading full text...

Full text loading...

References

  1. Allan, L.G.
    (1980) A note on measurement of contingency between two binary variables in judgment tasks. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 15, 147–149. doi: 10.3758/BF03334492
    https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03334492 [Google Scholar]
  2. Allport, D. , & Funnell, E
    (1981) Components of the mental lexicon. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. B, Biological Sciences, 295(1077), 397–410. doi: 10.1098/rstb.1981.0148
    https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.1981.0148 [Google Scholar]
  3. Anderson, J.R.
    (1983) A spreading activation theory of memory. Journal of Verbal Learning & Verbal Behavior, 22(3), 261–295. doi: 10.1016/S0022‑5371(83)90201‑3
    https://doi.org/10.1016/S0022-5371(83)90201-3 [Google Scholar]
  4. (2009) Cognitive psychology and its implications (7th edition). New York: Worth Publishers.
    [Google Scholar]
  5. Arnon, I. , & Snider, N
    (2010) More than words: Frequency effects for multi-word phrases. Journal of Memory and Language, 62, 67–82. doi: 10.1016/j.jml.2009.09.005
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2009.09.005 [Google Scholar]
  6. Audacity Team
    (2014) Audacity(Version 2.0.2): audacity.sourceforge.net/.
  7. Baayen, R.H. , Feldman, L.B. , & Schreuder, R
    (2006) Morphological influences on the recognition of monosyllabic monomorphemic words. Journal of Memory and Language, 55(2), 290–313. doi: 10.1016/j.jml.2006.03.008
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2006.03.008 [Google Scholar]
  8. Balota, D. , Cortese, M. , Sergent-Marshall, S.D. , Spieler, D. , & Yap, M
    (2004) Visual word recognition of single-syllable words. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 133(2), 283–316. doi: 10.1037/0096‑3445.133.2.283
    https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-3445.133.2.283 [Google Scholar]
  9. Balota, D. , Ferraro, F. , & Connor, L
    (1991) On the early influence of meaning in word recognition: A review of the literature. In P.J. Schwanenflugel (Ed.), The psychology of word meanings (pp.187–222). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
    [Google Scholar]
  10. Barsalou, L.W.
    (2010) Ad hoc categories. In P.C. Hogan (Ed.), The Cambridge encyclopedia of the language sciences (pp.87–88). New York: Cambridge University Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  11. Bates, D. , Maechler, M. , Bolker, B. , & Walker, S
    (2015) Fitting linear mixed-effects models using lme4. Journal of Statistical Software, 67(1), 1–48. doi: 10.18637/jss.v067.i01
    https://doi.org/10.18637/jss.v067.i01 [Google Scholar]
  12. Bencini, G.M.L. , & Goldberg, A
    (2000) The contribution of argument structure constructions to sentence meaning. Journal of Memory and Language, 43, 640–651. doi: 10.1006/jmla.2000.2757
    https://doi.org/10.1006/jmla.2000.2757 [Google Scholar]
  13. BNC
    (2007) BNC XML Editionwww.natcorp.ox.ac.uk/corpus/.
  14. Bod, R
    (2001) Sentence memory: Storage vs. computation of frequent sentences. Paper presented atthe CUNY-2001, Philadelphia, Pennsylvania.
    [Google Scholar]
  15. Brown, S
    (2014) xACT 2.22.
    [Google Scholar]
  16. Catlin, J
    (1973) In defense of sophisticated-guessing theory. Psychological Review, 80, 412–416. doi: 10.1037/h0035438
    https://doi.org/10.1037/h0035438 [Google Scholar]
  17. Cattell, J.M.
    (1886) The time it takes to see and name objects. Mind, 11, 63–65. doi: 10.1093/mind/os‑XI.41.63
    https://doi.org/10.1093/mind/os-XI.41.63 [Google Scholar]
  18. Davies, M
    (2004–) BYU-BNC: The British National Corpusavailable online atcorpus.byu.edu/bnc.
  19. (2008–) The corpus of contemporary American English: 450 million words, 1990–present. Available online atcorpus.byu.edu/coca/.
    [Google Scholar]
  20. de Nooy, W. , Mrvar, A. , & Batagelj, V
    (2010) Exploratory social network analysis with Pajek. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  21. Dehaene, S. , & Changeux, J.P.
    (2011) Experimental and theoretical approaches to conscious processing. Neuron Review, 70, 200–227. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.03.018
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2011.03.018 [Google Scholar]
  22. Durrant, P. , & Doherty, A
    (2010) Are high-frequency collocations psychologically real? Investigating the thesis of collocational priming. Corpus Linguistics and Linguistic Theory, 6, 125–155. doi: 10.1515/cllt.2010.006
    https://doi.org/10.1515/cllt.2010.006 [Google Scholar]
  23. Ebbinghaus, H
    (1885) Memory: A contribution to experimental psychology (trans. by H.A. Ruger & C.E. Bussenius [1913]) New York: Teachers College, Columbia.
    [Google Scholar]
  24. Ellis, N.C.
    (Ed.) (1994) Implicit and explicit learning of languages. San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  25. (1996) Sequencing in SLA: Phonological memory, chunking, and points of order. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 18(1), 91–126. doi: 10.1017/S0272263100014698
    https://doi.org/10.1017/S0272263100014698 [Google Scholar]
  26. (2001) Memory for language. In P. Robinson (Ed.), Cognition and second language instruction (pp.33–68). New York: Cambridge University Press. doi: 10.1017/CBO9781139524780.004
    https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9781139524780.004 [Google Scholar]
  27. (2002) Frequency effects in language processing: A review with implications for theories of implicit and explicit language acquisition. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 24(2), 143–188.
    [Google Scholar]
  28. (2006a) Language acquisition as rational contingency learning. Applied Linguistics, 27(1), 1–24. doi: 10.1093/applin/ami038
    https://doi.org/10.1093/applin/ami038 [Google Scholar]
  29. (2006b) Selective attention and transfer phenomena in SLA: Contingency, cue competition, salience, interference, overshadowing, blocking, and perceptual learning. Applied Linguistics, 27(2), 1–31. doi: 10.1093/applin/ami038
    https://doi.org/10.1093/applin/ami038 [Google Scholar]
  30. (2008) Usage-based and form-focused language acquisition: The associative learning of constructions, learned-attention, and the limited L2 endstate. In P. Robinson & N.C. Ellis (Eds.), Handbook of cognitive linguistics and second language acquisition (pp.372–405). London: Routledge.
    [Google Scholar]
  31. (2012) Formulaic language and second language acquisition: Zipf and the phrasal teddy bear. Annual Review of Applied Linguistics, 32, 17–44. doi: 10.1017/S0267190512000025
    https://doi.org/10.1017/S0267190512000025 [Google Scholar]
  32. (in press). On-line processing of verb-argument constructions: Lexical decision and semantic processing. Language and Cognition.
    [Google Scholar]
  33. Ellis, N.C. , & Ferreira-Junior, F
    (2009) Constructions and their acquisition: Islands and the distinctiveness of their occupancy. Annual Review of Cognitive Linguistics, 7, 111–139. doi: 10.1075/arcl.7.05ell
    https://doi.org/10.1075/arcl.7.05ell [Google Scholar]
  34. Ellis, N.C. , Frey, E. , & Jalkanen, I
    (2009) The psycholinguistic reality of collocation and semantic prosody (1): Lexical access. In U. Römer & R. Schulze (Eds.), Exploring the lexis-grammar interface (pp.89–114). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. doi: 10.1075/scl.35.07ell
    https://doi.org/10.1075/scl.35.07ell [Google Scholar]
  35. Ellis, R. , Loewen, S. , Erlam, R. , Philp, J. , & Reinders, H
    (2009) Implicit and explicit knowledge in second language learning, testing and teaching. Bristol, UK: Multilingual Matters.
    [Google Scholar]
  36. Ellis, N.C. , & O’Donnell, M.B.
    (2011) Robust language acquisition: An emergent consequence of language as a complex adaptive system. In L. Carlson , C. Hölscher , & T. Shipley (Eds.), Proceedings of the 33rd Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society (pp.3512–3517). Austin, TX: Cognitive Science Society.
    [Google Scholar]
  37. (2012) Statistical construction learning: Does a Zipfian problem space ensure robust language learning?In J. Rebuschat & J. Williams (Eds.), Statistical learning and language acquisition (pp.265–304). Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.
    [Google Scholar]
  38. Ellis, N.C. , O’Donnell, M.B. , & Römer, U
    (2012) Usage-based language: Investigating the latent structures that underpin acquisition. Currents in Language Learning, 1, 25–51.
    [Google Scholar]
  39. (2014) The processing of Verb-Argument Constructions is sensitive to form, function, frequency, contingency, and prototypicality. Cognitive Linguistics, 25, 55–98. doi: 10.1515/cog‑2013‑0031
    https://doi.org/10.1515/cog-2013-0031 [Google Scholar]
  40. Ellis, N.C. , & Ogden, D.C.
    (2015) Language cognition. Journal of Child Language, 42, 182–186. doi: 10.1017/S0305000914000646
    https://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000914000646 [Google Scholar]
  41. Forster, K.I. , & Chambers, S
    (1973) Lexical access and naming time. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 12, 627–635. doi: 10.1016/S0022‑5371(73)80042‑8
    https://doi.org/10.1016/S0022-5371(73)80042-8 [Google Scholar]
  42. Fox, J
    (2003) Effect displays in R for generalised linear models. Journal of Statistical Software, 8(15), 1–27. doi: 10.18637/jss.v008.i15
    https://doi.org/10.18637/jss.v008.i15 [Google Scholar]
  43. Francis, G. , Hunston, S. , & Manning, E
    (Eds.) (1996) Grammar patterns 1: Verbs. The COBUILD series. London: Harper Collins.
    [Google Scholar]
  44. Goldberg, A.E.
    (1995) Constructions: A construction grammar approach to argument structure. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  45. Goldberg, A.E. , & Bencini, G.M.L
    (2005) Support from processing for a constructional approach to grammar. In A.E. Tyler , M. Takada , Y. Kim , & D. Marinova (Eds.), Language in use: Cognitive and discourse perspectives on language and language learning (pp.3–18). Washington DC: Georgetown University Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  46. Gries, S.T. , & Ellis, N.C.
    (2015) Statistical measures for usage-based linguistics. Currents in Language Learning, 2, 228–255. doi: 10.1111/lang.12119
    https://doi.org/10.1111/lang.12119 [Google Scholar]
  47. Gries, S.T. , & Wulff, S
    (2005) Do foreign language learners also have constructions?: Evidence from priming, sorting, and corpora. Annual Review of Cognitive Linguistics, 3, 182–200. doi: 10.1075/arcl.3.10gri
    https://doi.org/10.1075/arcl.3.10gri [Google Scholar]
  48. Harnad, S
    (Ed.) (1987) Categorical perception: The groundwork of cognition. New York: Cambridge University Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  49. Howes, D.H. , & Solomon, R.L.
    (1951) Visual duration threshold as a function of word-probability. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 41(6), 401–410. doi: 10.1037/h0056020
    https://doi.org/10.1037/h0056020 [Google Scholar]
  50. Lakoff, G
    (1987) Women, fire, and dangerous things: What categories reveal about the mind. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. doi: 10.7208/chicago/9780226471013.001.0001
    https://doi.org/10.7208/chicago/9780226471013.001.0001 [Google Scholar]
  51. MacWhinney, B
    (1987) The competition model. In B. MacWhinney (Ed.), Mechanisms of language acquisition (pp.249–308). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
    [Google Scholar]
  52. Marcel, A.J.
    (1983) Conscious and unconscious perception: Experiments on visual masking and word recognition. Cognitive Psychology, 15(2), 197–237. doi: 10.1016/0010‑0285(83)90009‑9
    https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0285(83)90009-9 [Google Scholar]
  53. McDonough, K. , & De Vleeschauwer, J
    (2012) Prompt type frequency, auditory pattern discrimination, and EFL learners’ production of wh-questions. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 34(3), 355–377. doi: 10.1017/S0272263112000113
    https://doi.org/10.1017/S0272263112000113 [Google Scholar]
  54. Meyer, D.E. , & Schvaneveldt, R.W.
    (1971) Facilitation in recognizing pairs of words: Evidence of a dependence between retrieval operations. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 90, 227–234. doi: 10.1037/h0031564
    https://doi.org/10.1037/h0031564 [Google Scholar]
  55. Miller, G.A.
    (2009) WordNet: About us. RetrievedMarch 1, 2010, fromPrinceton Universitywordnet.princeton.edu
  56. Miller, G.A. , & Selfridge, J.A.
    (1950) Verbal context and the recall of meaningful material. The American Journal of Psychology, 63, 176–185. doi: 10.2307/1418920
    https://doi.org/10.2307/1418920 [Google Scholar]
  57. Morton, J
    (1969) Interaction of information in word recognition. Psychological Review, 76(2), 165–178. doi: 10.1037/h0027366
    https://doi.org/10.1037/h0027366 [Google Scholar]
  58. Peirce, J.W.
    (2007) PsychoPy: Psychophysics software in Python. Journal of Neuroscience Methods, 162(1), 8–13. doi: 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2006.11.017
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jneumeth.2006.11.017 [Google Scholar]
  59. R Development Core Team
    (2012) R: A language and environment for statistical computing. Vienna: R Foundation for Statistical Computing.
    [Google Scholar]
  60. Rescorla, R.A.
    (1968) Probability of shock in the presence and absence of CS in fear conditioning. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 66, 1–5. doi: 10.1037/h0025984
    https://doi.org/10.1037/h0025984 [Google Scholar]
  61. Robinson, P. , & Ellis, N.C.
    (Eds.) (2008) Handbook of cognitive linguistics and second language acquisition. London: Routledge.
    [Google Scholar]
  62. Römer, U. , O’Donnell, M.B. , & Ellis, N.C.
    (2015) Using COBUILD grammar patterns for a large-scale analysis of verb-argument constructions: Exploring corpus data and speaker knowledge. In M. Charles , N. Groom , & S. John (Eds.), Corpora, grammar, text and discourse: In honour of Susan Hunston (pp.43–72). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. doi: 10.1075/scl.73.03rom
    https://doi.org/10.1075/scl.73.03rom [Google Scholar]
  63. Rosch, E. , & Mervis, C.B.
    (1975) Cognitive representations of semantic categories. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 104, 192–233. doi: 10.1037/0096‑3445.104.3.192
    https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-3445.104.3.192 [Google Scholar]
  64. Rosch, E. , Mervis, C.B. , Gray, W.D. , Johnson, D.M. , & Boyes-Braem, P
    (1976) Basic objects in natural categories. Cognitive Psychology, 8, 382–439. doi: 10.1016/0010‑0285(76)90013‑X
    https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0285(76)90013-X [Google Scholar]
  65. Segalowitz, N
    (2010) The cognitive bases of second language fluency. New York: Routledge.
    [Google Scholar]
  66. Seidenberg, M.S. , & McClelland, J.L.
    (1989a) A distributed, developmental model of word acquisition. Psychological Review, 94, 523–568. doi: 10.1037/0033‑295X.96.4.523
    https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.96.4.523 [Google Scholar]
  67. (1989b) A distributed, developmental model of word recognition and naming. Psychological Review, 96(4), 523–568. doi: 10.1037/0033‑295X.96.4.523
    https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.96.4.523 [Google Scholar]
  68. Shanks, D.R.
    (1995) The psychology of associative learning. New York: Cambridge University Press. doi: 10.1017/CBO9780511623288
    https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511623288 [Google Scholar]
  69. Thorndike, E.L. , & Lorge, I
    (1944) The teacher’s book of 30,000 words. New York: Teacher’s College.
    [Google Scholar]
  70. Tremblay, A. , Derwing, B. , Libben, G. , & Westbury, C
    (2011) Processing advantages of lexical bundles: Evidence from self-paced reading and sentence recall casks. Language Learning, 61(2), 569–613. doi: 10.1111/j.1467‑9922.2010.00622.x
    https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9922.2010.00622.x [Google Scholar]
  71. Trousdale, G. , & Hoffmann, T
    (Eds.) (2013) Oxford handbook of construction grammar. Oxford: Oxford University Press. doi: 10.1093/oxfordhb/9780195396683.001.0001
    https://doi.org/10.1093/oxfordhb/9780195396683.001.0001 [Google Scholar]
  72. Zipf, G.K.
    (1935) The psycho-biology of language: An introduction to dynamic philology. Cambridge, MA: The M.I.T. Press.
    [Google Scholar]
/content/journals/10.1075/rcl.14.1.05ell
Loading
This is a required field
Please enter a valid email address
Approval was successful
Invalid data
An Error Occurred
Approval was partially successful, following selected items could not be processed due to error