1887
Volume 9, Issue 1
  • ISSN 2210-4372
  • E-ISSN: 2210-4380
USD
Buy:$35.00 + Taxes

Abstract

Abstract

This paper describes a structural account of phonetic symbolism and submits it to empirical investigation. To enable testing for possible iconic sound–emotion relations, participants compared pairs of syllables (e.g.,  – ) as well as pairs of emotional states (e.g., joyful – sad) on various perceptual scales (e.g., softness). In addition, we replicated the classic ‘bouba/kiki’ experiment to investigate sound-shape symbolism. In accordance with the theoretical model, the results of the experimental tasks suggest that participants can detect abstract similarities between speech sounds and emotions as well as geometrical shapes. We discuss the theoretical model and the experimental results in relation to previous empirical findings and conflicting evidence from the study of affective iconicity in poetry.

Loading

Article metrics loading...

/content/journals/10.1075/ssol.19002.gaf
2020-02-04
2025-02-19
Loading full text...

Full text loading...

References

  1. Aryani, A. , Conrad, M. , Schmidtke, D. , & Jacobs, A.
    (2018) Why “piss” is ruder than “pee”? The role of sound in affective meaning making. Plos One, 13(6). doi:  10.1371/journal.pone.0198430
    https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0198430 [Google Scholar]
  2. Aryani, A. , Hsu, C. T. , & Jacobs, A. M.
    (2018) The sound of words evokes affective brain responses. Brain Sciences, 8(6). doi:  10.3390/brainsci8060094
    https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci8060094 [Google Scholar]
  3. Aryani, A. , & Jacobs, A.
    (2018) Affective congruence between sound and meaning of words facilitates semantic decision. Behavioral Sciences, 8(6), 56. doi:  10.3390/bs8060056
    https://doi.org/10.3390/bs8060056 [Google Scholar]
  4. Aryani, A. , Kraxenberger, M. , Ullrich, S. , Jacobs, A. M. , & Conrad, M.
    (2016) Measuring the basic affective tone of poems via phonological saliency and iconicity. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 10(2), 191–204. doi:  10.1037/aca0000033
    https://doi.org/10.1037/aca0000033 [Google Scholar]
  5. Auracher, J. , Albers, S. , Zhai, Y. , Gareeva, G. , & Stavniychuk, T.
    (2011) P is for happiness, N is for sadness: Universals in sound iconicity to detect emotions in poetry. Discourse Processes, 48(1), 1–25. doi:  10.1080/01638531003674894
    https://doi.org/10.1080/01638531003674894 [Google Scholar]
  6. Bachorowski, J.-A.
    (1999) Vocal expression and perception of emotion. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 8(2), 53–57. 10.1111/1467‑8721.00013
    https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-8721.00013 [Google Scholar]
  7. Bänziger, T. , & Scherer, K. R.
    (2005) The role of intonation in emotional expressions. Speech Communication, 46, 252–267. doi:  10.1016/j.specom.2005.02.016
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.specom.2005.02.016 [Google Scholar]
  8. Benjamini, Y. , & Hochberg, Y.
    (1995) Controlling the false discovery rate: A practical and powerful approach to multiple testing. Journal of the Royal Statistical Society. Series B (Methodological), 57(1), 289–300. doi: 10.2307/2346101
    https://doi.org/10.2307/2346101 [Google Scholar]
  9. Cohen, J.
    (1992) A power primer. Psychological Bulletin, 112(1), 155–159. 10.1037/0033‑2909.112.1.155
    https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.112.1.155 [Google Scholar]
  10. De Saussure, F.
    (1916) Course in general linguistics. London, United Kingdom: Duckworth.
    [Google Scholar]
  11. Fónagy, I.
    (1961) Communication in poetry. Word – Journal of the International Linguistic Association, 17(2), 194–218.
    [Google Scholar]
  12. IBM Corp.
    IBM Corp. (2012) IBM SPSS Statistics for Windows, Version 21.0. Armonk, NY: IBM Corp.
    [Google Scholar]
  13. Johnson-Laird, P. N. , & Oatley, K.
    (1989) The language of emotions: An analysis of a semantic field. Cognition & Emotion, 3(2), 81–123. doi:  10.1080/02699938908408075
    https://doi.org/10.1080/02699938908408075 [Google Scholar]
  14. Knoeferle, K. , Li, J. , Maggioni, E. , & Spence, C.
    (2017) What drives sound symbolism? Different acoustic cues underlie sound-size and sound-shape mappings. Scientific Reports, 7(1), 1–11. doi:  10.1038/s41598‑017‑05965‑y
    https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-017-05965-y [Google Scholar]
  15. Köhler, W.
    (1947) Gestalt psychology (2nd ed.). New York, NY: Liveright.
    [Google Scholar]
  16. Kraxenberger, M. , & Menninghaus, W.
    (2016) Mimological reveries? Disconfirming the hypothesis of phono-emotional iconicity in poetry. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 1–9. doi:  10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01779
    https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01779 [Google Scholar]
  17. Pallant, J.
    (2007) One-way analysis of variance. SPSS Survival Manual: A step by step guide to data analysis using SPSS for Windows (3rd ed.). Maidenhead, United Kingdom: Open University Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  18. Ramachandran, S. , & Hubbard, E. M.
    (2001) Synaesthesia – A window into perception, thought and language. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 8(12), 3–34. doi:  10.1111/1468‑0068.00363
    https://doi.org/10.1111/1468-0068.00363 [Google Scholar]
  19. Russell, J. A.
    (1980) A circumplex model of affect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 39(6), 1161–1178. doi:  10.1037/h0077714
    https://doi.org/10.1037/h0077714 [Google Scholar]
  20. Sauter, D. A. , Eisner, F. , Calder, A. J. , & Scott, S. K.
    (2010) Perceptual cues in nonverbal vocal expressions of emotion. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 63(11), 2251–2272. doi:  10.1080/17470211003721642
    https://doi.org/10.1080/17470211003721642 [Google Scholar]
  21. Tsur, R.
    (2012) Playing by ear and the tip of the tongue: Precategorial information in poetry. Amsterdam, The Netherlands: John Benjamins. 10.1075/lal.14
    https://doi.org/10.1075/lal.14 [Google Scholar]
  22. Ullrich, S. , Aryani, A. , Kraxenberger, M. , Jacobs, A. M. , & Conrad, M.
    (2017) On the relation between the general affective meaning and the basic sublexical, lexical, and inter-lexical features of poetic texts-a case study using 57 Poems of H. M. Enzensberger. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 1–19. doi:  10.3389/fpsyg.2016.02073
    https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.02073 [Google Scholar]
  23. Ullrich, S. , Kotz, S. A. , Schmidtke, D. S. , Aryani, A. , & Conrad, M.
    (2016) Phonological iconicity electrifies: An ERP study on affective sound-to-meaning correspondences in German. Frontiers in Psychology, 7(AUG). doi:  10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01200
    https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01200 [Google Scholar]
  24. Whissell, C.
    (1999) Phonosymbolism and the emotional nature of sounds: Evidence of the preferential use of particular phonemes in texts of differing emotional tone. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 89(5), 19. doi:  10.2466/PMS.89.5.19‑48
    https://doi.org/10.2466/PMS.89.5.19-48 [Google Scholar]
  25. Wiseman, M. , & van Peer, W.
    (2002) The sound of meaning: An empirical study. In S. Csábi & J. Zerkowitz (Eds.), Textual secrets: The message of the medium (pp.379–383). Budapest, Hungary: Akadémiai Nyomda Martonvásár.
    [Google Scholar]
/content/journals/10.1075/ssol.19002.gaf
Loading
/content/journals/10.1075/ssol.19002.gaf
Loading

Data & Media loading...

This is a required field
Please enter a valid email address
Approval was successful
Invalid data
An Error Occurred
Approval was partially successful, following selected items could not be processed due to error