1887
Volume 6, Issue 2
  • ISSN 2210-4372
  • E-ISSN: 2210-4380
USD
Buy:$35.00 + Taxes

Abstract

This study tested the hypothesis that features of linguistically non-mandatory phonological recurrence (rhyme, alliteration, assonance, and consonance), parameters of word positioning (position within a line and line position) and dominant stress peaks are related to readers’ identification of distinctively joyful and sad words in poetry. To this end, forty-eight participants read eight German poems, completed an underlining task, and filled out a brief questionnaire. Results show that these target features are clearly of importance for readers’ perception of pronounced levels of joy and sadness. Words featuring alliteration, assonance or consonance were significantly more often underlined as distinctively joyful than were words that lack these features. Our study shows also that words that feature a dominant stress peak and are placed in more advanced positions within the poems were more likely to be identified as emotional (distinctively joyful and sad) when compared to words in earlier and unstressed positions.

Loading

Article metrics loading...

/content/journals/10.1075/ssol.6.2.06kra
2017-03-20
2025-04-22
Loading full text...

Full text loading...

References

  1. Albers, S.
    (2008) Lautsymbolik in ägyptischen Texten. Mainz: Philipp Von Zabern Verlag.
    [Google Scholar]
  2. Aristotle
    Aristotle ([ca. 335 BC] 2005) Poetics. Newburyport, MA: Focus.
    [Google Scholar]
  3. Aryani, A. , Jacobs, A. M. , & Conrad, M.
    (2013) Extracting Salient Sublexical Units from Written Texts:“EMOPHON”, a Corpus-Based Approach to Phonological Iconicity. Frontiers in Psychology, 4. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00654
    https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00654 [Google Scholar]
  4. Aryani, A. , Kraxenberger, M. , Ullrich, S. , Jacobs, A. M. , & Conrad, M.
    (2016) Measuring the basic affective tone of poems via phonological saliency and iconicity. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 10(2), 191–204. doi: 10.1037/aca0000033
    https://doi.org/10.1037/aca0000033 [Google Scholar]
  5. Auracher, J. , Albers, S. , Zhai, Y. , Gareeva, G. , & Stavniychuk, T.
    (2010) P is for Happiness, N is for Sadness: Universals in Sound Iconicity to Detect Emotions in Poetry. Discourse Processes, 48, 1–25. doi: 10.1080/01638531003674894
    https://doi.org/10.1080/01638531003674894 [Google Scholar]
  6. Baayen, R. H. , Davidson, D. J. , & Bates, D. M.
    (2008) Mixed-effects modeling with crossed random effects for subjects and items. Journal of Memory & Language, 59(4), 390–412. doi: 10.1016/j.jml.2007.12.005
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2007.12.005 [Google Scholar]
  7. Charness, G. , Gneezy, U. , & Kuhn, M. A.
    (2012) Experimental methods: Between-subject and within-subject design. Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization, 81(1), 1–8. doi: 10.1016/j.jebo.2011.08.009
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jebo.2011.08.009 [Google Scholar]
  8. Cutler, A.
    (1976) Phoneme-monitoring reaction time as a function of preceding intonation contour. Perception & Psychophysics, 20(1), 55–60. doi: 10.3758/bf03198706
    https://doi.org/10.3758/bf03198706 [Google Scholar]
  9. Damasio, A. R.
    (1994) Descartes’ Error: Emotion, Reason, and the Human Brain. New York: Grosset/Putnam.
    [Google Scholar]
  10. Demmerling, C. , & Landweer, H.
    (2007) Philosophie der Gefühle: Von Achtung bis Zorn. Stuttgart: Metzler.
    [Google Scholar]
  11. Ehrenfels, C. v.
    (1937) On Gestalt-qualities. Psychological Review, 44(6), 521–524. doi: 10.1037/h0056968
    https://doi.org/10.1037/h0056968 [Google Scholar]
  12. Ekman, P.
    (1992) An Argument for Basic Emotions. Cognition & Emotion, 6(3–4), 169–200. doi: 10.1080/02699939208411068.
    https://doi.org/10.1080/02699939208411068 [Google Scholar]
  13. Fónagy, I.
    (1961) Communication in poetry. Word, 17(2), 194–218. doi: 10.1080/00437956.1961.11659754
    https://doi.org/10.1080/00437956.1961.11659754 [Google Scholar]
  14. Genette, G.
    (1976/1995) Mimologics: Voyage en Cratylie. Lincoln: University of Nebraska.
    [Google Scholar]
  15. Hanauer, D.
    (1996) Integration of phonetic and graphic features in poetic text categorization judgements. Poetics, 23(5), 363–380. doi: 10.1016/0304‑422x(95)00010‑h
    https://doi.org/10.1016/0304-422x(95)00010-h [Google Scholar]
  16. Huron, D. B.
    (2006) Sweet anticipation: Music and the psychology of expectation: MIT press.
    [Google Scholar]
  17. Jacobs, A. M. , Lüdtke, J. , & Meyer-Sickendiek, B.
    (2013) Bausteine einer Neurokognitiven Poetik: Foregrounding/Backgrounding, lyrische Stimmung und ästhetisches Gefallen. Elements of a neurocognitive poetics: Foregrounding/backgrounding, typical mood, and aesthetic pleasure) in Stimmung und Methode (Mood and Method), ed. B. Meyer-Sickendiek and F. Reents (Tübingen: Mohr Siebeck), 63–94.
    [Google Scholar]
  18. Jacobs, A. M.
    (2015) Towards a neurocognitive poetics model of literary reading. Towards a cognitive neuroscience of natural language use, 135–159. doi: 10.1017/cbo9781107323667.007
    https://doi.org/10.1017/cbo9781107323667.007 [Google Scholar]
  19. Jakobson, R.
    (1960) Closing statement: Linguistics and poetics. Style in language, 350, 377.
    [Google Scholar]
  20. Jakobson, R. , & Waugh, L.
    (1979) The sound shape of language. London: Bloomington.
    [Google Scholar]
  21. Jespersen, O.
    (1933) Linguistica : selected papers in English, French and German. Copenhagen: Levin & Munksgaard.
    [Google Scholar]
  22. Kraxenberger, M. , & Menninghaus, W.
    (2016) Mimological Reveries? Disconfirming the Hypothesis of Phono-Emotional Iconicity in Poetry. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01779
    https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01779 [Google Scholar]
  23. Lausberg, H.
    (2008) Handbuch der Rhetorik: Eine Grundlegung der Literaturwissenschaft. Stuttgart: Franz Steiner Verlag.
    [Google Scholar]
  24. Lea, R. B. , Rapp, D. N. , Elfenbein, A. , Mitchel, A. D. , & Romine, R. S.
    (2008) Sweet Silent Thought Alliteration and Resonance in Poetry Comprehension. Psychological Science, 19(7), 709–716. doi: 10.1111/j.1467‑9280.2008.02146.x
    https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02146.x [Google Scholar]
  25. Liberman, M. , & Prince, A.
    (1977) On stress and linguistic rhythm. Linguistic inquiry, 8(2), 249–336.
    [Google Scholar]
  26. Menninghaus, W. , Bohrn, I. C. , Altmann, U. , Lubrich, O. , & Jacobs, A. M.
    (2014) Sounds funny? Humor effects of phonological and prosodic figures of speech. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 8(1), 71. doi: 10.1037/a0035309
    https://doi.org/10.1037/a0035309 [Google Scholar]
  27. Menninghaus, W. , Wagner, V. , Wassiliwizky, E. , Jacobsen, T. , & Knoop, C. A.
    (2017). The emotional and aesthetic powers of parallelistic diction. Poetics (advanced online publication). doi: 10.1016/j.poetic.2016.12.001
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.poetic.2016.12.001 [Google Scholar]
  28. Meyer, L. B.
    (1961) Emotion and Meaning in Music. Chicago: University Of Chicago Press. doi: 10.7208/chicago/9780226521374.001.0001
    https://doi.org/10.7208/chicago/9780226521374.001.0001 [Google Scholar]
  29. Obermeier, C. , Menninghaus, W. , von Koppenfels, M. , Raettig, T. , Schmidt-Kassow, M. , Otterbein, S. , & Kotz, S.A.
    (2013) Aesthetic and emotional effects of meter and rhyme in poetry. Frontiers in psychology, 4, 10. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00010
    https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00010 [Google Scholar]
  30. Obermeier, C. , Kotz, S. A. , Jessen, S. , Raettig, T. , von Koppenfels, M. , & Menninghaus, W.
    (2016) Aesthetic appreciation of poetry correlates with ease of processing in event-related potentials. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 16(2), 362–373. doi: 10.3758/s13415‑015‑0396‑x
    https://doi.org/10.3758/s13415-015-0396-x [Google Scholar]
  31. Pitt, M. A. , & Samuel, A. G.
    (1990) The use of rhythm in attending to speech. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 16(3), 564. doi: 10.1037//0096‑1523.16.3.564
    https://doi.org/10.1037//0096-1523.16.3.564 [Google Scholar]
  32. Quintilian
    Quintilian (1953) The Instituto oratoria of Quintilian. ( H. E. Butler , Ed. and trans.) London: W. Heinemann/Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  33. Reichel, U. D.
    (2012) PermA and Balloon: Tools for String Alignment and Text Processing. In: Proceedings of Interspeech, Portland, OR, paper346.
    [Google Scholar]
  34. Reichel, U. D. , & Kisler, T.
    (2014) Language-Independent Grapheme-Phoneme Conversion and Word Stress Assignment as a Web Service. In: R. Hoffmann (Ed.), Elektronische Sprachverarbeitung: Studientexte zur Sprachkommunikation71, pp.42–49. Dresden: TUDpress.
    [Google Scholar]
  35. Russell, J.A.
    (1980) A Circumplex Model of Affect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 39(6), 1161–1178. doi: 10.1037/h0077714
    https://doi.org/10.1037/h0077714 [Google Scholar]
  36. Schmitz, H.
    (1969) System der Philosophie: Der Gefühlsraum. Bonn: Bouvier.
    [Google Scholar]
  37. Schrott, R. , & Jacobs, A.
    (2011) Gehirn und Gedicht. Wie wir unsere Wirklichkeiten konstruieren. München: Carl Hanser.
    [Google Scholar]
  38. Selkirk L.
    (1984) Phonology and Syntax, MIT Press, Cambridge, Mass.
    [Google Scholar]
  39. Shields, J. L. , McHugh, A. , & Martin, J. G.
    (1974) Reaction time to phoneme targets as a function of rhythmic cues in continuous speech. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 102(2), 250. doi: 10.1037/h0035855
    https://doi.org/10.1037/h0035855 [Google Scholar]
  40. Tsur, R.
    (1992) What makes sound patterns expressive? The poetic mode of speech perception. Duke University Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  41. Valery, P.
    (1958) Remarks on Poetry. The Art of Poetry (trans. Denise Folliot ).
    [Google Scholar]
  42. Van Peer, W.
    (1986) Stylistics and psychology: Investigations of foregrounding. Taylor & Francis .
    [Google Scholar]
  43. (1990) The measurement of metre: Its cognitive and affective functions. Poetics, 19(3), 259–275. doi: 10.1016/0304‑422x(90)90023‑x
    https://doi.org/10.1016/0304-422x(90)90023-x [Google Scholar]
  44. Whissell, C.
    (2002) Emotion conveyed by sound in the poetry of Alfred, Lord Tennyson. Empirical Studies of the Arts, 20(2), 137–155. doi: 10.2190/6k4g‑lwpq‑ray8‑67qg
    https://doi.org/10.2190/6k4g-lwpq-ray8-67qg [Google Scholar]
  45. (2011) To those who feel rather than to those who think: Sound and emotion in Poe’s poetry. International Journal of English and Literature, 2(6), 149–156.
    [Google Scholar]
  46. Wiese, R.
    (1996) The Phonology of German. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  47. Wiseman, M. , & Van Peer, W.
    (2003) Roman Jakobsons Konzept der Selbstreferenz aus der Perspektive der heutigen Kognitionswissenschaft. In: H. Birus , S. Donat , & B. Meyer-Sickendiek (Eds.), Roman Jakobsons Gedichtanalysen, pp.277–306. Göttingen: Wallstein.
    [Google Scholar]
/content/journals/10.1075/ssol.6.2.06kra
Loading
/content/journals/10.1075/ssol.6.2.06kra
Loading

Data & Media loading...

  • Article Type: Research Article
Keyword(s): dominant stress peaks; joy; poetic phonology; poetry; sadness; underlining task; word position
This is a required field
Please enter a valid email address
Approval was successful
Invalid data
An Error Occurred
Approval was partially successful, following selected items could not be processed due to error