1887
Volume 19, Issue 1
  • ISSN 1387-6732
  • E-ISSN: 1570-6001
USD
Buy:$35.00 + Taxes

Abstract

In masked onset priming (MOPE) there is an overlap between prime and target in the onset. This has been shown to lead to faster word recognition of the target in adults in naming tasks but not in lexical decision tasks. To take a developmental stance, the present study investigated MOPE of various onsets in 30 adults and 74 second graders in a lexical decision task. For adults we found no effects for MOPE in lexical decision, which is congruent with the literature. However, results revealed priming effects for the beginning readers: accuracy scores were higher and response times were shorter for both words and pseudowords when there was an overlap of the onset between prime and target. The effects that we found may reflect either sublexical processing or speech planning, leaving it for future research to reach a firm conclusion with regard to underlying processes. Based on our findings, it can tentatively be assumed that beginning readers are susceptible to MOPE in lexical decision because of the fact that their word identification is far from automated.

Loading

Article metrics loading...

/content/journals/10.1075/wll.19.1.04van
2016-12-06
2024-09-12
Loading full text...

Full text loading...

References

  1. Acha, J. & M. Perea
    (2008) The effects of length and transposed-letter similarity in lexical decision: Evidence with beginning, intermediate, and adult readers. British Journal of Psychology99: 245–264. doi: 10.1348/000712607X224478
    https://doi.org/10.1348/000712607X224478 [Google Scholar]
  2. Carreiras, M. , L. Ferrand , J. Grainger & M. Perea
    (2005) Sequential effects of phonological priming in visual word recognition. Psychological Science16: 585–589. doi: 10.1111/j.1467‑9280.2005.01579.x
    https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2005.01579.x [Google Scholar]
  3. Castles, A. , C. Davis , P. Cavalot & K. Forster
    (2007) Tracking the acquisition of orthographic skills in developing readers: Masked priming effects. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology97: 165–182. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2007.01.006
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2007.01.006 [Google Scholar]
  4. Coltheart, M. , K. Rastle , C. Perry , R. Langdon & J. Ziegler
    (2001) DRC: A dual route cascaded model of visual word recognition and reading aloud. Psychological Review108: 204–256. doi: 10.1037/0033‑295X.108.1.204
    https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.108.1.204 [Google Scholar]
  5. Dimitropoulou, M. , J. A. Duñabeitia & M. Carreiras
    (2011) Influence of prime lexicality, frequency, and pronounceability on the masked onset priming effect. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology63: 1813–1837. doi: 10.1080/17470210903540763
    https://doi.org/10.1080/17470210903540763 [Google Scholar]
  6. Forster, K. I. & C. Davis
    (1991) The density constraint on form-priming in the naming task: Interference effects from a masked prime. Journal of Memory and Language30: 1–25. doi: 10.1016/0749‑596X(91)90008‑8
    https://doi.org/10.1016/0749-596X(91)90008-8 [Google Scholar]
  7. Goldinger, S. D. , P. A. Luce & D. B. Pisoni
    (1989) Priming lexical neighbors of spoken words: Effects of competition and inhibition. Journal of Memory and Language28: 501–518. doi: 10.1016/0749‑596X(89)90009‑0
    https://doi.org/10.1016/0749-596X(89)90009-0 [Google Scholar]
  8. Grainger, J. & L. Ferrand
    (1996) Masked orthographic and phonological priming in visual word recognition and naming: Cross-task comparisons. Journal of Memory and Language35: 623–647. doi: 10.1006/jmla.1996.0033
    https://doi.org/10.1006/jmla.1996.0033 [Google Scholar]
  9. Grainger, J. , B. Lété , D. Bertrand , S. Dufau & J. C. Ziegler
    (2012) Evidence for multiple routes in learning to read. Cognition123: 280–292. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2012.01.003
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2012.01.003 [Google Scholar]
  10. Kinoshita, S
    (2000) The left-to-right nature of the masked onset effect in naming. Psychonomic Bulletin & Reviews7: 133–141. doi: 10.3758/BF03210732
    https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03210732 [Google Scholar]
  11. Kinoshita, S. & A. Woollams
    (2002) The masked onset priming effect in naming: Computation of phonology or speech-planning. Memory & Cognition30: 237–245. doi: 10.3758/BF03195284
    https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03195284 [Google Scholar]
  12. Lété, B. & M. Fayol
    (2013) Substituted-letter and transposed-letter effects in a masked priming paradigm with French developing readers and dyslexics. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology114: 47–62. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2012.09.001
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2012.09.001 [Google Scholar]
  13. Marinus, E. & P. F. De Jong
    (2010) Size does not matter, frequency does: Sensitivity to orthographic neighbors in normal and dyslexic readers. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology106: 129–144. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2010.01.004
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2010.01.004 [Google Scholar]
  14. Martens, V. E. G. & P. F. De Jong
    (2006) The effect of word length on lexical decision in dyslexic and normal reading children. Brain and Language98: 140–149. doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2006.04.003
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandl.2006.04.003 [Google Scholar]
  15. Mousikou, P. , M. Coltheart , M. Finkbeiner & S. Saunders
    (2010) Can the dual-route cascaded computational model of reading offer a valid account of the masked onset priming effect?The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology63: 984–1003. doi: 10.1080/17470210903156586
    https://doi.org/10.1080/17470210903156586 [Google Scholar]
  16. Schaerlaekens, A. , D. Kohnstamm , M. Lejaegere , A. de Vries , L. Peeters & I. Zink
    (1999) Streeflijst woordenschat voor 6-jarigen: Derde en herziene versie gebaseerd op nieuw onderzoek in Nederland en Belgë. [Vocabulary achievement in six-year-olds in the Netherlands and Belgium]. Lisse, Belgium: Swets & Zeitlinger.
    [Google Scholar]
  17. Schafer, J. L. & J. W. Graham
    (2002) Missing data: Our view of the state of the art. Psychological Methods7: 147–177. doi: 10.1037/1082‑989X.7.2.147
    https://doi.org/10.1037/1082-989X.7.2.147 [Google Scholar]
  18. Schiller, N. O
    (1998) The effect of visually masked syllable primes on the naming latencies of words and pictures. Journal of Memory and Language39: 484–507. doi: 10.1006/jmla.1998.2577
    https://doi.org/10.1006/jmla.1998.2577 [Google Scholar]
  19. (2004) The onset effect in word naming. Journal of Memory and Language50: 477–490. doi: 10.1016/j.jml.2004.02.004
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2004.02.004 [Google Scholar]
  20. (2008) The masked onset priming effect in picture naming. Cognition106: 952–962. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2007.03.007
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2007.03.007 [Google Scholar]
  21. Schiller, N. O. & S. Kinoshita
    (2007) The effect of mismatching segmental information on the masked onset priming effect (MOPE). In International Congress of Phonetic Science (ICPhS) XVI , Saarbrucken, August (Vol. 6, No. 10).
    [Google Scholar]
  22. Schmalz, X. , E. Marinus & A. Castles
    (2013) Phonological decoding or direct access? Regularity effects in lexical decisions of Grade 3 and 4 children. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology66: 338–346. doi: 10.1080/17470218.2012.711843
    https://doi.org/10.1080/17470218.2012.711843 [Google Scholar]
  23. Schultz, T. & M. Wand
    (2010) Modeling coarticulation in EMG-based continuous speech recognition. Speech Communication52: 341–353. doi: 10.1016/j.specom.2009.12.002
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.specom.2009.12.002 [Google Scholar]
  24. Schreuder, R. & W. H. Van Bon
    (1989) Phonemic analysis: Effects of word properties. Journal of Research in Reading12: 59–78. doi: 10.1111/j.1467‑9817.1989.tb00303.x
    https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9817.1989.tb00303.x [Google Scholar]
  25. Share, D. L
    (1995) Phonological recoding and self-teaching: Sine qua non of reading acquisition. Cognition55: 151–218. doi: 10.1016/0010‑0277(94)00645‑2
    https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0277(94)00645-2 [Google Scholar]
  26. (2004) Orthographic learning at a glance: On the time course and developmental onset of selfteaching. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology87: 267–298. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2004.01.001
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2004.01.001 [Google Scholar]
  27. Van den Boer, M. , P. F. de Jong & M. M. Haentjens-van Meeteren
    (2012) Lexical decision in children: Sublexical processing or lexical search?The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology65: 1214–1228. doi: 10.1080/17470218.2011.652136
    https://doi.org/10.1080/17470218.2011.652136 [Google Scholar]
  28. Verhoeven, L
    (1995) Drie-minuten-toets. Handleiding. [Three Minute Test. Manual]. Arnhem, The Netherlands: Cito.
    [Google Scholar]
  29. Zeguers, M. H. T. , P. Snellings , H. M. Huizenga & M. W. Van der Molen
    (2014) Time course analyses of orthographic and phonological priming effects during word recognition in a transparent orthography. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology67: 1925–1943. doi: 10.1080/17470218.2013.879192
    https://doi.org/10.1080/17470218.2013.879192 [Google Scholar]
  30. Ziegler, J. C. , C. Perry , A. M. Jacobs , A. My-Wyatt , D. Ladner & G. Schulte-Krne
    (2003) Developmental dyslexia in different languages: Language-specific or universal?Journal of Experimental Child Psychology86: 169–193. doi: 10.1016/S0022‑0965(03)00139‑5
    https://doi.org/10.1016/S0022-0965(03)00139-5 [Google Scholar]
  31. Zoccolotti, P. , M. De Luca , E. Di Pace , F. Gasperini , A. Judica & D. Spinelli
    (2005) Word length effect in early reading and in developmental dyslexia. Brain and Language93: 269–373. doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2004.10.010
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandl.2004.10.010 [Google Scholar]
/content/journals/10.1075/wll.19.1.04van
Loading
/content/journals/10.1075/wll.19.1.04van
Loading

Data & Media loading...

This is a required field
Please enter a valid email address
Approval was successful
Invalid data
An Error Occurred
Approval was partially successful, following selected items could not be processed due to error