- Home
- e-Journals
- Constructions and Frames
- Previous Issues
- Volume 12, Issue 2, 2020
Constructions and Frames - Volume 12, Issue 2, 2020
Volume 12, Issue 2, 2020
-
Valency coercion in Italian
Author(s): Lucia Busso, Alessandro Lenci and Florent Perekpp.: 171–205 (35)More LessAbstractThe paper investigates valency coercion effects in Italian by means of an acceptability rating task on nine argument structure constructions. The experimental design follows Perek & Hilpert (2014) in presenting three conditions: grammatical, impossible and coercion stimuli. This design allows us to test several factors: the acceptability of creative coerced structures, the role of age and – most importantly – the influence of the construction itself. Results overall confirm our hypotheses: valency coercion is identified as an intermediate level between grammaticality and ungrammaticality, with varying degrees of “coercibility” across constructions. An influence of age is not in evidence for coercion sentences, suggesting that the systematic variation in acceptability is due to the influence of different constructions. We propose that coercion resolution results from the interaction of constructional and lexical semantics.
-
Reinforcement by realignment in diachronic construction grammar
Author(s): Yueh Hsin Kuopp.: 206–238 (33)More LessAbstractThis paper suggests that xiē ‘some’ in Mandarin Chinese originated as a quantifier but became a classifier in the yi ‘one’ construction via realignment, or change in inheritance in diachronic construction grammar. This change has created yi xiē, semantically equivalent to xiē, therefore it is also a case of reinforcement in the sense of Jespersen’s Cycle. However, this study argues that yi xiē has not necessarily undergone grammaticalisation. Generalising the analysis, two types of reinforcement are proposed: reinforcement by innovation and by realignment. The former involves grammaticalisation, but the latter may not. The study highlights the importance of higher-level generalisations in language change.
-
The growth of the transitivising Reaction Object Construction
Author(s): Tamara Bousopp.: 239–271 (33)More LessAbstractThis paper explores the growth of the Reaction Object Construction (ROC) as in Pauline smiled her thanks, offering new insights into its characterisation and historical development from the perspective of Construction Grammar (Goldberg 1995, 2006, 2019) and its application to patterns of language change (Hilpert 2013; Traugott & Trousdale 2013). It is argued that the modern ROC qualifies as a traditional form-meaning pairing and, at a deeper level, as a polysemous construction that follows the path of development of other transitivising constructions such as the way-construction (Israel 1996), and of processes of constructionalisation in general. Once the ROC imposes a coreferential constraint on its object argument, acquiring in this way its status as a form-meaning pairing over the Early Modern English period (1500–1700), the construction increases its productivity and schematicity; at the same time it decreases its compositionality since the link between the form/syntax and the overall meaning of the construction becomes less transparent, as in The door jingled a welcome. The ROC can thus be argued to be part and result of a broader development in the grammar of English, namely the historical trend towards transitivisation.
-
Moving Time vs. Frame-relative motion
Author(s): Kevin Ezra Moorepp.: 272–314 (43)More LessAbstractThere is an elaborate analogy between Moving Time (composed of primary metaphors; e.g. Christmas is approaching) and Frame-relative Fictive Motion (e.g. Your destination is approaching). It has been suggested that this analogy could be involved in the motivation of Moving Time. However, a semantic frame analysis that includes all stages of the motion event shows that this analogy could not be involved in the motivation of Moving Time. It is further argued that Moving Time and Frame-relative Fictive Motion are instances of different types of cognitive-semantic structure. Moving Time is a selective integration of concepts from frames that do not share elements with each other, whereas Frame-relative Fictive Motion presupposes a single semantic frame. For the purpose of distinguishing fictive motion from primary metaphor (e.g. Moving Time), Coextension-path and Pattern-path fictive motion are studied in addition to Frame-relative. These three types of fictive motion can be distinguished from primary metaphor because they involve the integration of concepts from frames that share specific structure, whereas primary metaphor involves frames that do not share specific structure.
In a preliminary classification of fictive motion as a type of metaphor, all three types of fictive motion discussed may be classified as resemblance-based metaphors. Coextension-path and Frame-relative fictive motion are also motivated by correlations in experience. These correlations, however, are different in kind from those that motivate primary metaphor.
-
K. Aaron Smith & Dawn Nordquist (Eds.) (2018). Functionalist and Usage-based Approaches to the Study of Language. In honor of Joan L. Bybee
Author(s): Jiyoung Yoonpp.: 315–326 (12)More LessThis article reviews Functionalist and Usage-based Approaches to the Study of Language. In honor of Joan L. Bybee
Most Read This Month

-
-
Change in modal meanings
Author(s): Martin Hilpert
-
-
-
Cascades in metaphor and grammar
Author(s): Oana David, George Lakoff and Elise Stickles
-
-
-
What is this, sarcastic syntax?
Author(s): Laura A. Michaelis and Hanbing Feng
-
- More Less